|
Citation |
- Permanent Link:
- http://ufdc.ufl.edu/UFE0041870/00001
Material Information
- Title:
- Cultural Experience From Studying Abroad Will It Enhance Creative Thinking?
- Creator:
- Lee, Christine
- Place of Publication:
- [Gainesville, Fla.]
- Publisher:
- University of Florida
- Publication Date:
- 2010
- Language:
- english
- Physical Description:
- 1 online resource (52 p.)
Thesis/Dissertation Information
- Degree:
- Master's ( M.A.E.)
- Degree Grantor:
- University of Florida
- Degree Disciplines:
- Educational Psychology
Human Development and Organizational Studies in Education
- Committee Chair:
- Therriault, David
- Committee Members:
- Linderholm, Tracy A.
- Graduation Date:
- 8/7/2010
Subjects
- Subjects / Keywords:
- Chinese culture ( jstor )
Creativity ( jstor ) Cross cultural psychology ( jstor ) Cross cultural studies ( jstor ) Cultural diversity ( jstor ) Cultural studies ( jstor ) Cultural values ( jstor ) International students ( jstor ) Multiculturalism ( jstor ) Study abroad ( jstor ) Human Development and Organizational Studies in Education -- Dissertations, Academic -- UF abroad, creativity, cultural, experience, study
- Genre:
- Electronic Thesis or Dissertation
bibliography ( marcgt ) theses ( marcgt ) Educational Psychology thesis, M.A.E.
Notes
- Abstract:
- There is a growing need for cross-cultural competency in today?s diversifying society. Cultural experiences from study abroad programs have found to be an effective means of providing students with an opportunity to develop valuable skills associated with cultural understanding and awareness. The present study investigated a possible cognitive benefit of studying abroad, focusing on creative thinking. In addition, this study investigated the nature of creativity, aiming to shed some light on the debate in regards to whether creativity is a domain general or domain specific construct. The relationship between cultural experience gained from studying abroad and creative thinking was assessed in a sample of undergraduate and graduate students (n=90) from the University of Florida. The Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults (ATTA) and the Cultural Creativity Test (CCT) were administered and analyzed between three groups: study abroad, plan to study abroad, and no study/no plan. An ANOVA analysis showed that cultural experience predicted enhanced creative thinking only on the CCT, indicating that creativity is a domain specific construct. Findings from this study indicate that a cognitive benefit gained from studying abroad is the facilitation of creative problem-solving abilities in culturally diverse settings. ( en )
- General Note:
- In the series University of Florida Digital Collections.
- General Note:
- Includes vita.
- Bibliography:
- Includes bibliographical references.
- Source of Description:
- Description based on online resource; title from PDF title page.
- Source of Description:
- This bibliographic record is available under the Creative Commons CC0 public domain dedication. The University of Florida Libraries, as creator of this bibliographic record, has waived all rights to it worldwide under copyright law, including all related and neighboring rights, to the extent allowed by law.
- Thesis:
- Thesis (M.A.E.)--University of Florida, 2010.
- Local:
- Adviser: Therriault, David.
- Statement of Responsibility:
- by Christine Lee.
Record Information
- Source Institution:
- University of Florida
- Holding Location:
- University of Florida
- Rights Management:
- Applicable rights reserved.
- Embargo Date:
- 10/8/2010
- Resource Identifier:
- 004979636 ( ALEPH )
769020231 ( OCLC )
- Classification:
- LD1780 2010 ( lcc )
|
Downloads |
This item has the following downloads:
|
Full Text |
CHAPTER 6
DISCUSSION
General Discussion
Based on my review of the literature examining cultural experience and creativity, I
hypothesized that students who have undergone a study abroad experience (Study Abroad group)
will produce significantly higher quality creative ideas on the measure of cultural creativity
assessed by the Cultural Creativity Task (CCT) compared to students who have not participated
in a study abroad program (including both students who have not studied abroad but plan to
studying abroad; Plan to Study group, and students who have not studied abroad and do not plan
on studying abroad; No Study/No Plan group). My findings showed that students who studied
abroad did in fact outperform the students who have not studied abroad on the CCT. These
significant differences between the Study Abroad group and the No Study/No Plan group are in
line with previous studies that showed enhanced creative abilities among students who have been
exposed to a culture different from their own compared to students who have experienced only
one culture (Kharkhurin, 2005; Leung & Chiu, 2008; Leung et. al, 2008, Lubart, 1990; Miranda,
1999). In addition, the significant difference between the Study Abroad group and the Plan to
Study group strengthens my conclusion that cultural experience is the variable which accounts
for increased creativity in the cultural domain. It was assumed that individuals in both the Study
Abroad and Plan to Study Abroad groups are interested in foreign cultures; however, the results
show that the actual experience of immersing oneself in a foreign culture has found to be related
to increased cultural creativity while interest in foreign cultures without the cultural experience is
not. This is consistent with the literature on multiculturalism and creativity which states that
cultural experience gained from living abroad is qualitatively different than other cultural
experiences such as travel or visits (Leung et al., 2008).
unfamiliar cultures with differing beliefs, attitudes, and values has shown to lead to the
development of multiple conceptual frameworks which provides individuals with a rich bank of
cultural information. This allows individuals to sample from a broad range of ideas during the
creative process. In addition, the process of integrating and synthesizing multiple cultural
worldviews has been linked to cognitive complexity. Cognitively complex abilities facilitate the
generation and innovative use of ideas during the creative process which results in highly
creative products. Finally, studies on biculturals show that the cognitive complexity acquired
from their experiences with multiple cultures are limited to tasks in the cultural domain.
between these two groups was made to account for differences that may have existed among
individuals who have not studied abroad. For example, interest in cross-cultural experiences was
a factor that was present in the Plan to Study group and absent in the No Study/No Plan group,
but it did not make a significant difference on the two groups' performance on either the general
creativity or cultural creativity tasks. The lack of significant differences between these two
groups on both tasks indicates that the cultural experience from study abroad programs is the
critical factor for increasing creativity in the cultural domain. The aim of the present study was to
focus on the relationship between cultural experience and creativity. The findings from my study
provides further evidence that a positive relationship between the cultural experience gained
from study abroad programs and enhanced creativity exists, particularly in respect to the cultural
domain of creativity.
Implications for Cultural Experience and Creative Thinking
There is a growing awareness for the need of cross-cultural competency in today's
diversifying society. The cultural experiences gained from studying abroad has consistently
proven to be an effective means in developing cultural and personal growth in individuals who
undergo such experiences (Carlson & Widaman, 1988; Carsello & Greaser, 1976; Kitsantas,
2004; Kitsantas & Meyers, 2001; Kuh & Kauffman, 1984). In the current study, we have found
that creativity is yet another benefit from the cultural experiences gained by studying abroad.
Specifically, our study shows that by living abroad, individuals will gain skills that enhance their
performance on creativity tasks which require them to employ information from different
cultural worldviews to create novel and practical ideas or solutions. With the increasing need of
multicultural aptitude, creative ability in the cultural domain is an especially valuable skill to
acquire. Our study provides support that study abroad programs provide students with the
cultural experiences that support creative development in the cultural domain. A significant
certain degree of expertise in the area. This is not easily achieved, often involving personal
challenges, struggles, and even resistance from within oneself or others. However, overcoming
the initial cognitive difficulties has shown to result in valuable cognitive skills and abilities that
lead to the production of quality creative ideas.
Finally, the current study has provided some insight in clarifying the construct of
creativity. The nature of creativity has been debated for decades. One of these debates revolves
around whether creativity is a domain-general or domain-specific construct. Many of the
arguments have been theoretical in nature (Amabile, 1996; Gardner, 183, 1999; Sternberg &
Lubart, 1995, 1996); however, recent research has begun to empirically examine the possible
multidimensional nature of creativity, focusing on the culture-specific domain (Benet-Martinez
et al., 2006; Tadmor & Tetlock, 2006; Tadmor et al., 2009). In line with previous findings, our
study suggests that a domain of cultural creativity exists. The mounting evidence that creativity
is in fact a domain-specific construct has implications for the assessment of creativity across all
types of creativity tasks. Multiple measures of creativity need to exist, targeting specific domains
of knowledge in order to accurately assess creativity. In addition, the findings from the current
study imply that any individual has the potential to be creative; however, their creative ability
will depend on the extent to which they are proficient with a specific domain and will be limited
to that domain. Future research is needed to identify additional domains creativity in order to
gain a deeper understanding of this construct.
Limitations and Directions for Future Research
Limitations in the current study include the selection of participants. All participants in the
study were chosen from the University of Florida, including undergraduate and graduate
students. Because of the highly educated participant sample, findings may not be generalizeable
to the rest of the population. Future research is needed to include individuals outside of the
college environment that have undergone cultural experience from living abroad so that our
findings may be applicable to a broader population.
The current study also did not account for the variability in the study abroad experiences
that individuals may have undergone. Factors such as the length of the study abroad program, the
country in which they lived abroad, and whether or not they lived with a host family were not
considered in the current study. Participants from the present study reported a wide range of
study abroad experiences. Countries in which students studied abroad included Australia,
Belgium, Germany, Hong Kong, Paris, Puerto Rico, India, Italy, Japan, London, Mexico, Spain,
Thailand, Yucatan Peninsula and a Semester at Sea which included visits to Spain, Italy, Croatia,
Greece, Turkey, Bosnia, Bulgaria, Egypt, and Morocco. The length of time spent studying
abroad ranged between 3 weeks to 4 months. Research regarding study abroad programs suggest
that depending on the cultural context, creativity can be encouraged or stifled across different
domains of creativity (e. g. arts, politics, economics, and religion) (Oner, 2000; Paletz & Peng,
2008; Rudowicz, 2003; Weiner, 2000). In addition, cultures may vary in the degree to which
divergent thinking versus conformity in thinking is encouraged or condemned (Miranda, 1999;
Rudowicz, 2003). Future research is needed to more carefully examine the variability across
study abroad experiences and the possible effects these variables may have on creativity. For
example, a possible area for future research is to investigate if there is a linear pattern in the
amount of cultural experience and creativity, or if there is a specific amount of time that the
individual must spend abroad before creative benefits begin to emerge.
There is also a concern with the measure used for assessing cultural creativity. The
measure used in the current study (CCT) was a broad assessment for determining whether or not
an individual possessed cultural creativity. The CCT has not been established as a valid and
differentiation, articulation, and abstraction on a given task (Benet-Martinez et al., 2006). It is
suggested that the higher creative ability among biculturals derive from their increased flexible
approach due to this dual perspective. The hypothesis was tested by presenting the participants
with either cultural or neutral pictures and having them write a description of the pictures. The
study found that Chinese American biculturals' free descriptions of both American and Chinese
cultures are higher in cognitive complexity than that of Anglo-American monoculturals. Findings
suggest that cultural frame switching may trigger complex cognitive processes that include
detecting, processing, and organizing diverse cultural meanings (Benet-Martinez et al., 2006).
Benet-Martinez et al. (2006) also proposed that the creative superiority (measured by
cognitive complexity) exhibited by biculturals are limited to tasks that contain cultural schemas
and cues. This proposition is in line with Tadmor et. al's (2009) study mentioned earlier, which
showed that biculturals exhibited greater integrative complexity on tasks of cultural complexity
over tasks of general complexity. It was argued that in culturally neutral domains where explicit
cultural references are not present, biculturals will not exhibit increased cognitive complexity
compared to monoculturals (Benet-Martinez et al., 2006). Findings from their study supported
this hypothesis. Chinese-American biculturals' representations of culture (Chinese or American)
were more cognitively complex (higher in density and abstractness) than those of monoculturals
largely specific to the cultural domain (Benet-Martinez et al., 2006). Benet-Martinez et al. (2006)
argue that biculturals' superiority on cognitive complexity tasks are present only on tasks that
allow them to draw from their knowledge bank of multiple cultural representations, and absent
on tasks outside of the cultural domain.
Based the literature reviewed on cultural diversity within groups and individuals, there is
considerable evidence that cultural diversity is positively linked to creativity. Exposure to
benefit of studying abroad seems to be the facilitation creative problem-solving abilities in
culturally diverse settings.
The current study also has broader implications for the benefits of cultural experience on
individuals' overall cognitive capacities. Studies of bicultural individuals show that the
experience of immersing oneself in a foreign culture provides a cognitively complex challenge.
Creativity includes many of the skills that are involved in cognitively complex tasks such as
differentiation and integration of ideas (Benet-Martinez et al., 2006; Tadmor & Tetlock, 2006;
Tadmor et al., 2009). Similar to cognitively complex cultural tasks, individuals who produce
high-quality culturally relevant creative ideas engage in the attainment and synthesis of two or
more cultural frames (Tadmor et al., 2009), and are able to more efficiently generate and explore
relevant information during the creative process (Leung et al., 2008; Rietzschel et al., 2007). In
the present study, we found evidence that these cognitively complex abilities are developed
within the culture-specific domain in which individuals have gained knowledge and experience
during their time abroad. However, implications for these findings can be applied to the creative
process outside of the cultural domain. To achieve the skills required to effectively utilize
information at a cognitively complex level in order to demonstrate creativity, individuals must
undergo in-depth experiences within specific domains. This journey requires a profound and
reflective immersion into a specific domain in order to notice and attempt to resolve all of the
nuances, exceptions, and contradictory information that may be present. For example, in the
present study, individuals who studied abroad were challenged to adapt their culturally familiar
script in order to incorporate their new cultural experience that contained foreign beliefs,
attitudes, and behaviors. A broad implication of the current study's findings is that the
achievement of a creative product in any domain is not possible without first developing a
CHAPTER 1
STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM
Introduction
Today's society is comprised of a rich blend of ethnically and culturally diverse groups.
Currently, approximately 30% of the U.S. population is made up of ethnic minority groups such
as African American, Latino/Hispanic, Asian-Pacific Islander, and Native American (U.S.
Census Bureau, 2000) and this trend towards diversity is on the rise. It is predicted that by the
year 2015, ethnic minorities will constitute approximately one third of the United States
population, and by the year 2050, students of color will consist of approximately 48 to 50% of
the school-age population (U.S. Census Bureau, 2000). The increasing diversity in our nation is
clearly represented in the growing numbers of individuals from different ethnic and cultural
groups within our schools and workplaces, resulting in more and more frequent interpersonal
communications among individuals of different backgrounds. As our nation develops into a
multicultural demographic, fostering an awareness and understanding of different worldviews is
a significant goal; this goal is especially important for our schools to accomplish in order to
produce open-minded and informed individuals. Although the need for cultural studies has been
recognized for some time, the implementation of effective cultural development curricula has
been limited (Miranda, 1999). One area showing movement in students' development of cultural
awareness is study abroad programs.
Benefits of Studying Abroad
Study abroad programs provide students with the opportunity to experience a culture
different from their own by immersing themselves in a foreign country. Study abroad programs
constitute all educational programs that occur in a foreign country outside of the geographical
boundaries of the country of origin (Carlson & Widaman, 1988; Kitsantas, 2004). Such programs
generality. For example, the abilities component, including analytic, creative, and practical
abilities, can range from domain-specific to domain-general or sometimes even cut across
domains (Sternberg, 2005). In contrast, the personality and knowledge components are more
domain-specific (Sternberg, 2005). In addition, the individual's creative attitudes (i.e., their
willingness to redefine difficult obstacles and to analyze solutions to problems), play an
important role in their creative ability in both domain-specific and domain-general aspects
(Sternberg & Lubart, 1995; Sternberg, 2005).
There is currently a dearth of information on the topic of whether creativity is a general
aptitude or an ability that is specific to a particular domain. Research on expertise can be drawn
from to shed some light on this matter. Ericsson and Charness (1994) investigated the structure
and acquisition of expertise, reviewing the works of influential philosophers and theorists
including Gardner (1983), Suzuki (1963, 1981), Young (1759), and Galton (1869/1979) who
believed that talent stemmed from inherited abilities. Although this has been the traditionally
held view on the nature of exceptional abilities among individuals, empirical evidence for innate
talent is lacking (Ericsson & Charness, 1994). In actuality, studies are showing that superior
performance in multiple domains including musical, athletic, spatial, mathematical, and
physiological activities are due not to innate ability, but rather, the accumulation of skills
acquired only through deliberate practice (Ericsson & Charness, 1994). Deliberate practice
requires individuals to engage in effortful, individualized structured learning that targets a
specific set of knowledge and skills relevant to a particular domain. Studies investigating experts
including master chess players, professional athletes and musicians provide evidence that
expertise is reached only by engaging in consistent, deliberate practice for an extended period of
time. The development of expertise is also facilitated under optimal environmental conditions
The creative cognitive approach views the creative process as a result of specific cognitive
operations on available knowledge (Rietzschel, Nijstad, & Stroebe, 2007). According to this
view, as long as the individual effectively utilizes knowledge from accessible domains, anyone
has the potential to produce creative outcomes (Leung et al., 2008; Rietzschel et al., 2007). The
creative process includes a generative process and an explorative process. In the generative
process, the individual actively retrieves or seeks out relevant information to generate ideas that
have creative potential. In the explorative process, the individual explores and inspects the ideas
to determine which should be further processed. The final ideas are modified, elaborated upon,
and transformed to produce a creative end result (Leung et al., 2008). In respect to the present
study's focus of investigation, the creative cognitive approach provides an explanation for the
positive link between cultural experience and creativity. In contrast to individuals who have
experience with only one culture and therefore have one highly accessible cultural category,
individuals who have adapted another culture have two or more highly accessible cultural
categories from which they can draw upon during the creative process. Therefore, these
individuals have an advantage during the creative process due to the broader range of knowledge
they can access from which they can generate and explore ideas.
The dynamic constructivist approach provides a framework for understanding the creative
process specifically in regards to using cultural knowledge (Chiu, Hong, Morris, & Menon,
2000; Morris & Fu, 2001). This view models cultural knowledge as a set of different knowledge
structures that can be activated by conditions such as priming (Chiu et al., 2000; Morris & Fu,
2001). When an individual encounters culturally relevant stimuli in a particular context, they are
able to access their cultural knowledge structures to retrieve and apply the information to
produce high-quality creative products (Hong, & Chiu, 2001; Hong et al., 2001; Hong, Benet-
CHAPTER 5
RESULTS
The Creativity Index (CI) scores from the Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults (ATTA)
were used to assess participants' creative abilities in respect to domain general creativity. The
first column on Table 1 presents the means and standard deviations of the CI scores for each of
the three groups (Study Abroad, Plan to Study, and No Study/No Plan). Results revealed that no
significant differences existed on the CI scores between the three groups; Study Abroad group
(M = 4.20, SD = 1.03), Plan to Study Abroad group (M = 3.87, SD = .97), and No Study/No Plan
group (M = 3.90, SD = 1.24), F (2, 87) =.85, p > .05 (See Table 5-1).
The scores from the Cultural Creativity Task (CCT) were used to assess participants'
creative abilities in the culture-specific domain. The second column of Table 1 presents the
means and standard deviations of the CCT scores for each of the three groups. Results revealed
that significant differences on the CCT scores existed between the three groups; Study Abroad
(M = 3.14, SD = .38), Plan to Study Abroad (M = 2.62, SD = .25), and No Study/No Plan (M =
2.59, SD = .21), F (2, 87) = 34.97, p < .05.
To further examine the significant differences between the three groups on the CCT, a
Tukey post-hoc analysis was performed. The analysis revealed that the Study Abroad group
significantly outperformed the Plan to Study group and the No Study/No Plan group on the CCT,
F (2, 87) = 34.97, p < .001. In addition, no significant difference existed between the Plan to
Study and No Study/No Plan groups in regards to the CCT scores.
Table 5-1. The means and standard deviations for the ATTA and CCT measures.
Condition ATTA CCT
M SD M SD
Study abroad 4.20 1.03 3.14* 0.38
Plan to study 3.87 .97 2.62* .25
No study/no plan 3.90 1.24 2.59* .38
p <. 05, Significant pairs
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This study would not have been possible without the support and encouragement of my
family, friends, and colleagues. I also wish to thank my committee members, David Therriault
and Tracy Linderholm, for their guidance and support.
was administered to a group of European American undergraduates to assess their level of
multicultural experiences. The MES consists of items including whether or not an individual
speaks a foreign language, the percentage of an individual's lifetime spent in a foreign country,
home country of an individual's parents, the extent of exposure the individual has had to other
cultures, their five favorite restaurants and the type of cuisine served, their five favorite
musicians, and their five favorite friends and their friends' nationalities (Leung & Chiu, 2008).
The participants were asked to write down the first five gift ideas that came to mind. Each
participant was given dominance/rank ratio score based on how readily the idea was generated
by all of the participants. Results showed that the extent of multicultural experiences was
negatively correlated with the average dominance/rank index for the gift ideas. Therefore, a link
between greater multicultural experience and the generation of more unconventional ideas was
found. Finally, a third study was conducted in which participants were asked to develop a
creative research idea. The participants were given the opportunity to draw from writings of 15
scholars (5 Americans, 5 Turks, and 5 Chinese) who had written on the nature of happiness
(Leung & Chiu, 2010). Results showed that individuals with greater multicultural experiences
chose a higher percentage of foreign sayings. Based on these findings, the authors suggest that
multicultural experience engage cognitive skills such as retrieval and integration of seemingly
unrelated ideas that support creative thinking (Leung & Chiu, 2010).
A qualitative study conducted by Miranda (1999) also found a relationship between
cultural experience and enhanced creativity. Four case studies using a naturalistic inquiry method
was used to investigate four individuals' experience in different cultures. The participants had
spent three or more months in a different culture, and were asked what they perceived were the
effects of the cultural experiences on their creative development. A major theme that emerged
TABLE OF CONTENTS
A CK N O W LED G M EN T S ........................................................... ........................................... 3
L IS T O F T A B L E S ................................................................................. 6
ABSTRAC T .........................................................................................
CHAPTER
1 STATEM EN T OF THE PROBLEM ....................................... .......................................9
In tro d u ctio n ................... ......................................................... 9
B benefits of Studying A broad .................................................................. ......................... 9
Defining Creativity ................. ............................ ..................... 11
M multicultural Experience and Creativity ................ ...... ..................... ............... ... 13
Creativity as a Domain-General or Domain-Specific Construct................. ................17
T he C reativ e P process ............... ................................................ .................. ........... ...... 20
Cultural D diversity during the Creative Process ........................................... .....................22
C cultural D diversity in G groups ........................................ ............................................23
Cultural D diversity in Individuals ......................................................................... 24
2 PU R PO SE O F TH E STU D Y ......................................................................... ...................28
3 HYPOTHESES ............. .................. .. ....... ............... ............. ... 30
4 M E T H O D ..........................................................................32
P articip an ts .........................................................................3 2
Measures ...................................................32
Measure of Domain General Creativity .............................. ...............33
M measure of Domain Specific Creativity ......................... .................... ........... 34
P ro cedu re ................................ ........................................................... 3 7
Data Collection ................................. ................................. ........ 37
S c o rin g ........................ ................... .. ........................................................................ 3 7
A n aly sis .............. .................................................................. 3 8
5 R E SU L T S .............. ... ................................................................39
6 D IS C U S S IO N ......................................... .. .......................................................4 0
G general D discussion ....... ...... .... ...............................................................40
Implications for Cultural Experience and Creative Thinking.................... ............... 42
Limitations and Directions for Future Research............................................... 44
LIST OF REFEREN CES ....................................................................................................... 47
4
were asked to draw as many pictures or objects as they can using the triangles. This activity
provides figural, fluency, originality, elaboration, and flexibility scores. Activities 2 and 3
provide 10 figural criterion-referenced responses. Each of the raw scores from the norm-
referenced measures is converted to a normalized scaled score which are summed to produce a
total scaled score. The criterion-referenced creativity indicators are scored on a three-point scale
of 0 to 2. The sum of the total scaled score and creative indicators yield a Creativity Index (CI)
(Goff & Torrance, 2002).
The Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults has been reviewed and found to be reliable and
valid measures of creative thinking (Chase, 1985). A meta-analysis of creative thinking included
only studies that used the ATTA in order to insure a valid and reliable measure (Rose & Lin,
1984). The ATTA has been normalized. The norms reported in the ATTA manual are based
upon adults who had completed the D-TTCT prior to the year 2000. The manual reports the
Kuder-Richardson (KR21) reliability coefficient for the total raw score for the four creative
abilities measured by the ATTA as .84, and for the total raw score with the creativity indicators
score as .90 (Goff & Torrance, 2000b). Inter-rater reliabilities range from .95 to .99.
Measure of Domain Specific Creativity
The Cultural Creativity Task (CCT): The CCT consists of five 3 minute brainstorming
prompts intended to target creativity in the cultural domain. Each prompt is located at the top of
an 8.5" x 11" piece of paper. Participants are given three minutes for each prompt, and are asked
to come up with as many ideas as possible for each of the five prompts in the CCT. The first
prompt included in the CCT is an altered version of The Tourism Problem (Taylor, Berry, &
Block, 1958). The Tourism Problem is a task developed for a brainstorming activity. It consists
of the following statement:
CULTURAL EXPERIENCE FROM STUDYING ABROAD: WILL IT ENHANCE
CREATIVE THINKING?
By
CHRISTINE S. LEE
A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL
OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT
OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF
MASTER OF ARTS IN EDUCATION
UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA
2010
the points assigned for the norm-referenced and criterion-referenced measures, and reached an
agreement. The rest of the ATTA tasks were coded. There was a moderate inter-rater reliability
coefficient (r = .76).
The standard deviation for the fluency scores of each of the five activities in the CCT was
determined for both ratings for each participant. The ratings were considered to be in agreement
for scoring fluency if the fluency score for each of the five activities fell within the average of
their individual standard deviations for each activity. We discussed and agreed upon the
necessary adjustments needed to be made to our coding procedures for originality. The
remainder of the tasks was coded. We were considered to be in agreement if our final creativity
score (sum of the originality scores divided by the sum of the fluency scores) for the CCT fell
within 1 point of each other. The average of the final CCT scores for each participant was used
for analysis. There was a strong inter-rater reliability coefficient (r = .89).
Once all of ninety ATTA and CCT tasks were coded, we met to discuss any remaining
disagreements for both tasks. A 100% agreement on the ATTA and CCT scores for every
participant was reached before the data was analyzed.
Analysis
Two one-way between groups ANOVAs were conducted to compare the effect of cultural
experience from studying abroad on domain-general creativity (Creativity Index scores) and
domain-specific creativity (Cultural Creativity Test scores). Where an overall omnibus F test
was significant, we employed a follow-up Tukey post-hoc test to identify specific difference
between the three groups with respect to our hypotheses. The criterion for significance was set at
an alpha level of .05.
from the interpretations of the data was that the cultural experiences provided the opportunity for
the individuals to be more adaptive, innovative, and divergent thinkers (Miranda, 1999). In
addition, the participants reported that divergent thinking and risk taking was encouraged in the
culture that had a diverse international community (Miranda, 1999). Multicultural experiences
have shown to cultivate creativity by exposing individuals to situations in which they are faced
with foreign cultures that challenge them to acquire alternative interpretations, seek out ideas
from diverse sources, and engage in cognitive exploration.
In a study related to the investigation of the link between cultural experience and
creativity, Kharkhurin (2005) found that cultural experience is closely tied to the introduction of
a new language. Language is inextricable linked to culture, as it shapes thought and guides a
person's understanding of the world. It allows people from different cultures to communicate
their unique worldviews with each other, affecting each other's constructions of reality
(Kharkhurin, 2005; Miranda, 1999). The cognitive processes involved in the acquisition of a
new language, therefore, overlap with the cognitive processes involved in accommodating a new
culture. The simultaneous attainment of two different language systems from two different
cultures has shown to impact individuals' cognitive abilities that are associated with creativity
(Kharkhurin, 2005; Miranda, 1999; Pearl & Lambert, 1962). In a study investigating the possible
roles of bilingualism and biculturalism on creativity, Kharkhurin (2005) found that factors
involved in learning a new language, including language color terms, language-specific
descriptors of personality, and verbal or figural modes of communication have all shown to
positively influence creativity. Two factors in bilinguals' development, including linguistic skills
and cross-cultural experience, were assessed using the Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults
(ATTA) composed of three divergent thinking tasks. Findings showed that both factors were
construct, but one that is influenced by internal, personal as well as external, environmental
factors. A widely used definition of creativity is that it is an interaction between ability and
process which results in a novel idea or work that is accepted as practical and appropriate by a
group of people (Amabile, 1982; Lubart, 1990; Niu & Sternberg, 2002; Plucker & Beghetto,
2004; Sternberg, 2006). Sternberg (1993) argued that creativity encompasses a wide range of
valuable skills that contribute to improving students' academic performance. Creative
individuals engage in decisions that involve redefining problems and exploring alternative
perspectives, analyzing one's thoughts, convincing other's to adopt one's ideas, overcoming
obstacles and taking risks, being patient and tolerant in times of ambiguity, and learning from
their mistakes (Sternberg, 1993). He stressed that these cognitive processes associated with
creativity are found to be beneficial in the academic domain.
In addition to being a cognitive process, creativity should be perceived as a cultural
phenomenon as well. In the Investment Theory of Creativity, Sternberg and Lubart (1991)
suggest that creativity can be understood as a combination of variables including abilities,
intellectual processes, knowledge, personality, motivation, and environmental variables which
includes the physical setting and culture of an individual. According to this theory, people are
"buying low and selling high" (Sternberg, 2006, p. 87) with respect to ideas. Creative individuals
will pursue ideas that are initially unknown or out of favor but then develop them to their
potential (Sternberg, 2006). Csikszentmihalyi (1988) proposed a dynamic systems perspective
which views the person as part of a system in which social, historical, and cultural factors
influence the individual's development and expression of creativity. In Csikszentmihalyi's
(1988) dynamic system, creativity involves the interaction between three forces: the culture, the
related to fluency, assessed by the ability to rapidly generate ideas and solutions to a problem,
and elaboration, assessed by the ability to engage in a detailed analysis of an idea and carry it out
(Kharkhurin, 2005). In addition, cross-cultural experience was also related to originality,
assessed by the amount of novel ideas generated on the divergent thinking tasks (Kharkhurin,
2005). These findings indicate that the cultural experience that accompanies the acquisition of a
new language plays a positive role on an individuals' creativity.
Overall, research has found creative benefits from experiences with new cultures, whether
it is through a living abroad experience, seeing images of different cultures, or learning a new
language. Cultural experiences provide cognitively challenging tasks of adapting and integrating
differing ideas, values, and behaviors from an unfamiliar worldview and have shown to produce
creative gains.
Creativity as a Domain-General or Domain-Specific Construct
Another challenge involved in the endeavor to define creativity revolves around the debate
over whether creativity is a domain general or a domain specific construct. In Frames of Mind,
Howard Gardner (1983, 1999) proposed a theory of multiple intelligence that views creativity as
a partially domain specific construct, consisting of eight distinct intelligence. Based on his
research studying the performance of prodigies and savants, Gardner (1983) concluded that an
innate capacity to rapidly learn material relevant to one of his proposed intelligence existed
among exceptionally talented individuals. According to Gardner, each intelligence can be relied
upon and applied in order to manifest into creativity. However, Gardner also believed that
particular intelligence can converge to produce creative outcomes (Sternberg, 2005), suggesting
that creativity can also be a domain-general construct made up of different combinations of his
proposed intelligence.
LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS
ATTA (Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults) Measure of domain-general
creativity.
CCT (Cultural Creativity Test) Measure of domain-specific creativity.
CI (Creativity Index) Final score of the Abbreviated Torrance Test for
Adults.
2006; Tadmor & Tetlock, 2006; Tadmor et al., 2009). Empirical research on experts from
varying domains revealed that the knowledge and skills acquired to demonstrate mastery was
limited to the domain in which the individuals had extensive practice and training (Ericsson &
Charness, 1994). Evidence for this hypothesis will suggest that creativity is a domain-specific
construct; that individuals' creative abilities depend on the degree of relevant experience and
knowledge they have accumulated for that specific domain.
In hypothesis 3, I propose that individuals who have not yet studied abroad but are
planning to study abroad will not differ on measures of cultural or general creativity compared to
individuals who have not and are also not planning to study abroad. Evidence for this hypothesis
will strengthen my predicted findings that cultural experience is the variable underlying
increased cultural creativity. This hypothesis accounts for the possibility that initial differences
in cultural creativity may exist between the individuals who plan to study abroad and the
individuals who do not plan to study abroad.
Biculturals who adapted two cultural worldviews were compared to individuals who had
abandoned their old culture and assimilated the new culture (assimilated individuals), and to
individuals who separated from the new culture and maintained their old culture (separated
individuals). Participants included 75 East-Asian undergraduate students. Ward and Kennedy's
(1994) Acculturation Index (AI) was administered to determine if the participants were going to
be in the bicultural, the assimilated, or the separated group. Integrative complexity was measured
by open-ended questions which were analyzed to determine the degree of differentiation and
integration on a 7-point scale. Two questions assessed cultural complexity (e. g., "What does it
mean to you to be bicultural? Would you define yourself as a bicultural?") and two questions
assessed general complexity (e. g., "Some people feel that organizations waste too much time
listening to different points of view and opinions during group meetings. Others feel they don't
spend enough time. How do you feel? What do you think should be the right balance?" (Tadmor
et al., 2009, p. 112). Results supported the benefits ofbiculturalism, showing that biculturals
were more integratively complex compared to assimilated and separated individuals (Tadmor et
al., 2009). In addition, integrative complexity among biculturals was greater for questions of
cultural complexity compared to questions of general complexity, indicating that level of
integrative complexity may depend on the domain of the task (Tadmor et al., 2009). Tadmor et
al. (2009) concluded that biculturals generally demonstrate greater cognitive complexity
compared to monoculturals; however, this difference is most pronounced when the task is
specific to the cultural domain.
Benet-Martinez et al. (2006) proposed that biculturals think about culture in more complex
ways than monoculturals, because they are forced to navigate between two cultural frameworks,
a process known as cultural frame switching. Cognitive complexity is measured by the degree of
an increased awareness of multicultural issues, but also an improvement in their personal well-
being including increased self-awareness, reflective thought, self-reliance, and self-confidence
(Kuh & Kaufman, 1984). In a study conducted by Kitsantas (2004), students were assessed
before and after their study abroad experience on four dimensions of the Cross-Cultural
Adaptability Inventory (CCAI) (Kelley & Meyers, 1995). The four dimensions assessed
emotional resilience, openness/flexibility, perceptual acuity, and personal autonomy (Kitsantas,
2004). The study found that in the final assessment, students reported higher scores on all four
dimensions.
Overall, our review of the research on study abroad programs suggests that these
programs promote cultural and personal growth in students. However, much of the work on
study abroad programs focuses upon the positive affective benefits (e. g., feelings towards
foreign countries, feelings of personal well-being) of studying abroad, and research that explores
the possible positive links between study abroad programs and cognitive processes remain
largely underdeveloped. This link is important to study because the ability to engage in
cognitively complex ways with regards to multicultural information is becoming an increasingly
valuable skill to develop in today's diversifying society. Therefore, the purpose of this study is to
investigate the benefits of undergoing a study abroad program on cognition, focusing specifically
on creative thinking. The growing field of research in creativity is showing that developing the
cognitive skills and abilities associated with creative thinking is linked to an enhanced ability to
navigate through every day social situations, as well as through academic and professional
realms.
Defining Creativity
Before reviewing the literature on the link between study abroad programs and creativity, it
is first necessary to define creativity. Creativity is difficult to define because it is not an isolated
individuals who have adapted more than one cultural framework. Also related to the present
study, the following section will provide evidence that a culture-specific domain of creativity
exist, supporting earlier theories that suggest that creativity is a multidimensional construct.
Cultural Diversity in Groups
Particularly in the field of management, research on cultural diversity in group work
indicates that there is value in having culturally diverse individuals work together on problem-
solving tasks. A study by Watson, Kumar, and Michaelsen (1993) explored the effects of high
cultural diversity on group interaction and group problem-solving. Participants consisted of
undergraduate students enrolled in a management course who were randomly assigned to either a
group in which all members were of the same nationality and ethnic background (homogeneous),
or a group in which at least two members were of different nationality and three members of a
different ethnic background (heterogeneous) (Watson et al., 1993). The groups were given case
studies over a 17 week period and required to analyze these real-life company situations (Watson
et al., 1993). The case study analyses were evaluated on four criteria: the range of perspectives,
the number of problems identified, alternatives generated, and the quality of the solutions
(Watson et al., 1993). The results showed that at the end of the 17 weeks, the heterogeneous
group were more effective on identifying problems, seeing different perspectives, and generating
multiple alternatives (Watson et al., 1993). A follow- up study conducted by McLeod et al.
(1996) investigated the difference in performance on a brainstorming task between all Anglo-
American (homogenous) groups and ethnically diverse (heterogeneous) groups consisting of
Anglo, Asian, African, and Hispanic Americans. Undergraduate and graduate students from
different academic majors were selected. All Anglo-American participants were randomly
assigned to either the homogenous or heterogeneous groups, and participants of other ethnicities
were randomly assigned to a heterogeneous group. The groups were given a brainstorming task
B IO G R A PH IC A L SK E T C H ............................................................................... .....................52
Measure of Domain General Creativity
Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults (ATTA; Goff & Torrance, 2002). The Torrance
Tests of Creative Thinking (TTCT; Torrance, 1966) is the standard measure for assessing
creative abilities. The TTCT requires about 45 minutes for the Verbal tests, and 30 minutes for
the Figural tests (TTCT; Torrance, 1966). The Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults (ATTA;
Goff & Torrance, 2002) is a converted form of the Demonstration Form of the Torrance Tests
(D-TTCT; Torrance, Wu, & Ando, 1980). The D-TTCT consists of activities utilizing the same
rationale as activities in the original TTCT, but in abbreviated form, requiring considerably less
testing time. The ATTA is used to assess participants' divergent thinking abilities. The ATTA
contains three 3-minute tasks scored based on four norm-referenced measures (fluency,
originality, elaboration, and flexibility) and 15 criterion-referenced indicators (e. g., richness and
Colorfulness of Imagery, Openness: Resistance to Premature Closure) (TTCT; Torrance, 1966).
The total scores from the norm-referenced measures and criterion-referenced indicators combine
to yield a Creativity Index (CI).
The ATTA begins with a written instruction that explains the general guidelines of the task
and encourage participants to use their imagination and thinking abilities. In the verbal
component of the task (Activity 1) participants are asked to suppose that they could walk on air
or fly without being in an airplane or similar vehicle, and then to identity the troubles they might
encounter. This activity provides verbal fluency and originality norm-referenced scores and five
verbal criterion-referenced responses. In the picture completion task (Activity 2), the participants
are presented with two incomplete figures and asked to draw pictures with the figures and make
the pictures as unusual as possible. This activity provides figural fluency, originality, and
elaboration norm-referenced scores. In the picture construction task (Activity 3) the participants
were presented with a group of nine identical isosceles triangles arranged in a 3 x 3 matrix and
Each year a great many American tourists go to Europe to visit. But now suppose
that our country wanted to get many more Europeans to come to America during
their vacations. What steps can you suggest that would get many more Europeans
to come to this country as tourists? (Taylor, Berry, & Block, 1958, p.28).
For the purposes of this study, the statement was altered:
Each year a great many American tourists go to foreign countries to visit. But now
suppose that our country wanted to get many more people from foreign countries to
come to America during their vacations. What steps can you suggest that would get
many more foreign people to come to this country as tourists?
This task is relevant to cultural creativity because of the international focus of the task. In
the original Tourism Problem task, the participants are required to spend 10 minutes to generate
as many ideas as they can on 8.5" x 11" paper. Taylor, Berry, and Block (1958) pre-tested a total
of about twenty problems with Yale students both individually and in small groups. From the
pre-tests, three problems (including the Tourism Problem) were selected based on findings that
showed these tests were most conducive for brainstorming, seemed to be of most interest to the
participants, and produced many and varied responses. McLeod, Lobel, and Cox Jr. (1996)
employed the Tourism Problem to study the relationship between ethnic diversity and creativity
in small groups.
The Tourism problem was administered to each small group, and the participants were
given 15 minutes to individually produce as many ideas as they could to get more tourists to visit
the United States 8.5" x 11" paper (McLeod et al., 1996). Each idea was then transferred to an
index card with a group identification code on the reverse side (McLeod et al., 1996). Redundant
responses were eliminated for each group's set of ideas. Each idea was then judged for
effectiveness and feasibility based on a 5-point scale. Effectiveness was defined as "how much
of a contribution the idea make toward the objective of getting more tourists to visit the U.S."
(Lamm & Trommsdorff, 1973, p. 363). The scale for effectiveness ranged from 1- would attract
hardly anyone to 5- would attract almost anyone (McLeod et al., 1996). Feasibility was defined
and creativity show that students who have studied abroad demonstrate increased interest in
travel, art, foreign languages, history, and architecture; domains that are all linked to creative
pursuits (Carsello & Creaser, 1976). In a study examining if creative performance results from
multicultural experience, European American undergraduates who had little knowledge of
Chinese culture were shown a 45-minute PowerPoint presentation and then asked to complete a
creativity test (Leung et al., 2008). The presentation included pictures, music, and videos based
on one of the following five conditions: unicultural Chinese culture condition which consisted of
Chinese arts, architectures, food and other elements from China; juxtaposition condition which
consisted of images from both Chinese and American cultures;fusion condition which consisted
of images that depicted the American-Chinese fusion culture; the American culture control
which consisted of images from the American cultures; and the no-slide control in which the
participants did not watch a presentation. The creativity task required participants to read a
summary of the original Cinderella story and rewrite a new version for Turkish children. Two
coders rated the creativity tasks on a 7-point Likert scale based on their subjective definition of
creativity. The participants in the conditions that exposed them to more than one culture (the
American and Chinese cultures in juxtaposition and the American-Chinese fusion conditions)
wrote more creative Cinderella stories compared to participants in the other three conditions who
were exposed to either one or no culture (Leung & Chiu, 2010). These findings showed that even
one hour of multicultural exposure may engage some cognitive skills that promote creative
thinking. In addition, these creative benefits were present five to seven days after the original
study indicating possible long-term effects of multicultural exposure. Leung & Chiu (2010) also
investigated the link between multicultural experience and the tendency to retrieve ideas from
diverse cultures and generate unconventional ideas. The Multicultural Experience Survey (MES)
LIST OF REFERENCES
Amabile, T. M. (1982). Social psychology of creativity: A consensual assessment
technique. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 43(5), 997-1013.
Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context: Update to "The Social Psychology of
Creativity. "Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Benet-Martinez, V., Lee, F., & Leu, J. (2006). Biculturalism and cognitive complexity: Expertise
in cultural representations. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 3 7, 386-407.
Bloom, B. S. (1985). Generalizations about talent development. In B. S. Bloom (Ed.),
Developing talent in young people (pp. 507-549). New York: Ballantine Books.
Carlson, J.S., & Widaman, K.F. (1988). The effects of study abroad during college on
attitudes toward other cultures. International Journal ofIntercultural Relations, 12 (1), 1-
18.
Carsello, C., & Greaser, J. (1976). How college students change during study abroad.
College Student Journal, 10, 276-278.
Chiu, C.-Y., Hong, Y.-Y., Morris, M. W., & Menon, T. (2000). Motivated cultural cognition:
The impact of implicit cultural theories on dispositional attribution varies as a function of
need for closure. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 78(2), 247-259.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1988). Society, culture and person: a system view of creativity. In
R.J. STERNBERG (Ed.) The Nature of Creativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Ericsson, K. A., & Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance: Its structure and
acquisition. American Psychologist, 49, 725-747.
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. Th., & Heizmann, S. (1993). Can we create gifted people? In
CIBA Foundation Symposium 178, The origins and development of high ability
(pp. 222-249). Chichester, England: Wiley.
Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of Mind: The Theory of Multiple Intelligences. Basic Books,
New York.
Gardner, H. (1999). Intelligence Reframed: Multiple Intelligences for the 21st Century.
Basic Books, New York.
Goff, K. & Torrance, E. P. (2000a). Abbreviated Torrance Test for adults
Manual. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service.
Goff, K. & Torrance, E. P. (2000b). Abbreviated Torrance Test for adults
Manual. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service.
2010 Christine S. Lee
CHAPTER 2
PURPOSE OF THE STUDY
The existing literature on study abroad programs provides substantial evidence that a
multitude of benefits exist from the cultural experiences gained while living abroad. Individuals
who have studied abroad demonstrate increased development in cultural understanding and
awareness as well as higher levels of overall personal well-being (Carlson & Widaman, 1988;
Kitsantas, 2004; Kuh & Kaufman, 1984; McCabe 1994). The purpose of this study is to pursue
another possible benefit from study abroad programs: creativity. The literature reviewed provides
evidence for the relationship between cultural experience and enhanced creativity. In the past,
this relationship has been studied by manipulating cultural experience through the use of cultural
images (Benet-Martinez et al., 2006; Leung et. al, 2008), conducting qualitative case studies of
individuals who have studied abroad (Miranda, 1999), and examining the relationship between
bilingualism and creativity (Kharkhurin, 2005). The aim of the present study is to contribute to
this area of research by integrating the findings on study abroad programs as well as the findings
on cultural experience and creativity by conducting an empirical study using study abroad
experiences as the criteria for having cultural experience to investigate the possible influence it
may have on creativity.
Significance for theory: Also related to the present study is the research on biculturals that
show a relationship between experiencing and adapting a new culture and increased cognitive
complexity (Benet-Martinez et al., 2006; Tadmor & Tetlock, 2006; Tadmor et al., 2009). I am
interested in this line of work due to the similar cognitive processes that exist in cognitively
complex tasks and creative tasks. However, these studies have been limited to studying
biculturals who are of another ethnicity, primarily first or second generation Asian-Americans.
The present study aims to extend these findings to a broader population by examining the
called the Tourism Problem (Taylor, Berry, & Block, 1958) in which participants were required
to formulate as many ideas as possible on how to attract more tourists to the United States.
Responses were rated in terms of their feasibility and effectiveness. In line with Watsons et al.'s
(1993) findings, results showed that ethnically heterogeneous groups produced higher quality
creative ideas that were significantly more feasible and more effective ideas compared to
ethnically homogeneous groups (McLeod et al., 1996). One explanation for this difference was
that the Tourism Problem is a culture-specific task in which the ethnically heterogeneous group
would have an advantage over the ethnically homogeneous group due to the diverse range of
cultural knowledge present in former group.
Cultural Diversity in Individuals
Research also supports the creative benefits of cultural diversity within an individual.
Studies investigating the creative benefits of cultural experience have compared biculturals to
monoculturals. Biculturals are individuals who have internalized two cultures, whereas
monoculturals have internalized a single culture (Benet-Martinez, Lee, & Leu, 2006). Research
on biculturals reveals that being exposed to a culture different from one's own facilitates the
development of cognitively complex skills that support the creative process. Empirical studies
examining bicultural individuals have found that creativity is found in relatively high rates
among individuals who are first or second generation immigrants and for immigrants who are
ethnically diverse (Lambert, Tucker, & d'Anglejan, 1973; Simonton, 1997, 1999).
In a study by Tadmor et al., (2009), the acculturation process and effects of second-culture
exposure on individuals' integrative complexity was investigated. Within the cultural domain,
integrative complexity was defined as the acknowledgement of differing cultural perspectives on
the same issue (differentiation), and the ability to integrate the two perspectives by finding
connections between two seemingly contrasting worldviews (integration) (Tadmor et al., 2009).
Hong, Y.-Y., Benet-Martinez, V., Chiu, C.-Y., & Morris, M. W. (2003). Boundaries of cultural
influence: Construct activation as a mechanism for cultural differences in social
perception. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34, 453-464.
Hong, Y.-Y., & Chiu, C.-Y. (2001). Toward a paradigm shift: From cross-cultural differences in
social cognition to social-cognitive mediation of cultural differences. Social Cognition,
19(3), 181-196.
Hong, Y.-Y., Ip, G., Chiu, C.-Y., Morris, M. W., & Menon, T. (2001). Cultural identity and
dynamic construction of the self: Collective duties and individual rights in Chinese and
American cultures. Social Cognition, 19(3), 251-268.
Kharkhurin, A. V. (2005). On the possible relationships between bilingualism,
biculturalism and creativity: A cognitive perspective. Ph.D. dissertation, City University
of New York, United States, New York. Retrieved May 23, 2009, from Dissertations &
Theses: Full Text database. (Publication No. AAT 3169934).
Kitsantas, A. (2004). Studying abroad: The role of college student's goals on the
development of cross-cultural skills and global understanding. College Student Journal,
Vol 38(3), 441-452.
Kitsantas, A. & Meyers, J. (2001, March). Studying abroad: Does it enhance college student
cross-cultural awareness? Paper presented at the combined annual meeting of the San
Diego State University and the U.S. Department of Education Centers for International
Business Education and Research, San Diego, CA.
Kuh, G.K., & Kauffman, N.F. (1984). The impact of study abroad on personal
development of college students. Bloomington, IN,: Indiana University School of
Education.
Lambert, W. E., Tucker, G.R., & d'Anglejan, A. (1973). Cognitive and attitudinal
consequences of bilingual schooling: The St. Lambert project through grade five. Journal
ofEducational Psychology, 65, 141-159.
Leung, A. K., & Chiu, C.-Y. (2008). Interactive effects of multicultural experiences and
openness to experience on creative potential. Creativity Research Journal, 20(4), 376-
382, 2008.
Leung, A. K., & Chiu, C.-Y. (2010). Multicultural experience, idea receptiveness, and
creativity. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, XX(X), 1-19.
Leung, A. K., Maddux, W. W., Galinksy, A. D., Chiu, C.-Y. (2008). Multicultural
experience enhances creativity. American Psychologist, 63(3), 169-181.
Plucker and Beghetto (2004) take a hybrid position on the debate of domain generality and
specificity, and argue that both domain specific and domain general features of creativity need to
be examined. Drawing from the body of research on general cognitive abilities and processes as
well as those focused on creativity and problem-solving (Carroll, 1993; Runco, 1994; Runco &
Okuda, 1988), the authors believe that creativity is possible across domains (Plucker & Beghetto,
2004). However, while creative ability may be a domain-general construct, the interest and
commitment that an individual must invest in a particular area is the domain-specific aspect of
the creative process. As an individual gains experience in a particular domain or task, they
become more domain-specific in their commitment, interest, time, and effort put towards that
domain. As a result, the individual is more likely to develop a creative product in that specific
domain. In addition, the context in which a product is evaluated for creativity includes both
domain-general and domain-specific aspects as well. A collective agreement within a society that
a product is novel and useful exists; however, there are also contextual boundaries within
particular domains that provide guidelines on whether a product is creative or not (e. g.,
assessing a science fair project versus assessing an invention for the Nobel Prize) (Plucker &
Beghetto, 2004). The authors conclude that the empirical definition of creativity views it as a
domain-general construct; however, features of creativity such as the process and evaluation of
creativity are domain-specific.
Other researchers suggest that creativity is a construct including both domain-specific and
domain-general features (Amabile, 1996; Sternberg & Lubart, 1995, 1996). In the Investment
Theory of creativity, Sternberg and Lubart (1995) state that creativity consists of multiple
components that include abilities, knowledge, thinking styles, personality, motivation, and
environmental variables. Each component differs in its degree of domain specificity and domain
CHAPTER 4
METHOD
Participants
A total of 90 students from a large southeastern university were recruited for this study. The
students were recruited from the university's International Center by means of the study abroad
listserv as well as from a research participants' pool. Participants consisted of 30 students who
have studied abroad (Study Abroad group), 30 students who have not studied abroad but are
planning on studying abroad in the future (Plan to Study group), and 30 students who have not
studied abroad and are not planning on studying abroad in the future (No Study/No Plan group).
The Study Abroad group participants (5 male and 25 female) were between 18 to 35 years of age
(M= 20.27, SD = 3.11), and consisted of 12 Caucasian/White, 3 African American, 5
Latino/Hispanic, 8 Asian American, and 2 Other students. The Plan to Study group participants
(5 male and 25 female) were between 18 to 21 years of age (M= 19.03, SD= .67), and consisted
of 19 Caucasian/White, 5 African American, 4 Latino/Hispanic, and 2 Other students. The No
Study/No Plan group participants (5 male and 25 female) were between 18 to 22 years of age (M
= 19.76, SD = 1.64), and consisted of 21 Caucasian/White, 4 African American, 3
Latino/Hispanic, and 2 Other students.
Measures
Based on the hypotheses proposed, two measures of creativity will be employed. The
Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults (ATTA; Goff & Torrance, 2002), was administered to
assess domain general creativity, and The Cultural Creativity Test was administered to assess
domain specific creativity in the cultural domain.
Lubart T. I. (1990). Creativity and cross-cultural variation. International Journal of
Psychology, 25, 39-59.
McCabe, L. T. (1994). The development of a global perspective during participation in
semester at sea: A comparative global education program. Educational Review, 46(3),
275-286.
McLeod, P. L., Lobel, S. A., & Cox, T. H. Jr. (1996). Ethnic diversity and creativity in small
groups. Small Group Research, 27(2), 248-264.
Miranda, E. L. M. (1999). Multiple cultural experiences: An exploration of their effects
and the fostering of creativity. Ph.D. dissertation, Texas A&M University, United States,
Texas. Retrieved May 23, 2009, from Dissertations & Theses: Full Text database.
(Publication No. AAT 9934461)
Morris, M. W., & Fu, H.-Y. (2001). How does culture influence conflict resolution? A
dynamic constructivist analysis. Social Cognition, 19(3), 324-349.
Niu, W., & Sternberg, R. (2002). Contemporary studies on the concept of creativity: The
East and the West. Journal of Creative Behavior, 36(4), 269-288.
Oner, B. (2000). Innovation and adaptation in a Turkish sample: a preliminary study. The
Journal of Psychology, 134, 671-676.
Paletz, S. B. F., & Peng, K. (2008). Implicit theories of creativity across cultures: Novelty
and appropriateness in two product domains. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39,
286-302.
Pearl, E. & Lamber, W. E. (1962). The relation of bilingualism to intelligence.
Psychological Monographs, 76, 23.
Plucker, J. A., & Beghetto, R. A. (2004). Why creativity is domain general, why it looks
domain specific, and why the distinction does not matter. Creativity: From potential to
realization. Sternberg, Robert J. (Ed); Grigorenko, Elena L. (Ed); Singer, Jerome L. (Ed);
pp. 153-167. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association, 2004. x, 226
pp.
Rietzschel, E. F., Nijstad, B. A., & Stroebe, W. (2007). Relative accessibility of domain
knowledge and creativity: The effects of knowledge activation on the quantity and
originality of generated ideas. Journal ofExperimental Social Psychology, 43(6), 933-
946.
Rudowicz, E. (2003). Creativity and culture: a two way interaction. Scandinavian Journal
ofEducational Research, 47(3), 2003.
Abstract of Thesis Presented to the Graduate School
of the University of Florida in Partial Fulfillment of the
Requirements for the Degree of Master of Arts in Education
CULTURAL EXPERIENCE FROM STUDYING ABROAD: WILL IT ENHANCE
CREATIVE THINKING?
By
Christine Lee
August 2010
Chair: David Therriault
Major: Educational Psychology
There is a growing need for cross-cultural competency in today's diversifying society.
Cultural experiences from study abroad programs have found to be an effective means of
providing students with an opportunity to develop valuable skills associated with cultural
understanding and awareness. The present study investigated a possible cognitive benefit of
studying abroad, focusing on creative thinking. In addition, this study investigated the nature of
creativity, aiming to shed some light on the debate in regards to whether creativity is a domain
general or domain specific construct. The relationship between cultural experience gained from
studying abroad and creative thinking was assessed in a sample of undergraduate and graduate
students (n=90) from the University of Florida. The Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults
(ATTA) and the Cultural Creativity Test (CCT) were administered and analyzed between three
groups: study abroad, plan to study abroad, and no study/no plan. An ANOVA analysis showed
that cultural experience predicted enhanced creative thinking only on the CCT, indicating that
creativity is a domain specific construct. Findings from this study indicate that a cognitive
benefit gained from studying abroad is the facilitation of creative problem-solving abilities in
culturally diverse settings.
relationship between cultural experience and creativity among any individuals who have adapted
a new cultural worldview through the study abroad experience. Therefore, this study will include
individuals who are biculturals not necessarily because of their ethnic background, but instead
due to their cultural experiences from studying abroad. Drawing from the study abroad and
multicultural fields of research, it is assumed that the cultural experience gained from living
abroad develops biculturalism among students as they immerse themselves in a foreign country
and adapt the values, attitudes, and behaviors from that new cultural worldview.
Finally, the present study aims to contribute to the ongoing debate concerning the nature of
creativity. Some researchers argue that creativity is a domain-general construct (Gardner, 1983,
1999; Plucker and Beghetto, 2004), while others argue that is domain-specific (Benet-Martinez
et al., 2006, Leung et al. 2008). There are also researchers that argue creativity can be both
domain-general and domain-specific (Amabile, 1996; Sternberg & Lubart, 1995, 1996; Tadmor
& Tetlock, 2006; Tadmor et al., 2009). Recent empirical studies conducted on biculturals point
to creativity being a domain-specific construct, specifically in regards to the cultural domain
(Benet-Martinez et al., 2006; Leung et al., 2008; Tadmor & Tetlock, 2006; Tadmor et al., 2009).
Research in the area of expertise also point to the abilities associated with creativity being
domain-specific in nature (Ericsson & Charness, 1994). The present study aims to provide
further clarification on the nature of creativity in regards to whether it is a domain-general of
domain-specific construct.
Shoshani, Y., & Hazi, R. B. (2007). The use of the internet environment for enhancing creativity.
Educational Media International, 44(1), 17-32.
Simonton, D. K. (1997). Foreign influence and national achievement: The impact of open
milieus on Japanese civilization. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 72, 86-
94.
Simonton, D. K. (1999). Origins of genius: Darwinian perspectives on creativity. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Sternberg, J. R. (2006). Creating a vision of creativity: The first 25 years. Psychology of
Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, S(1), 2-12.
Sternberg, J. R. (2005). Creativity or creativities? International Journal ofHuman-
Computer Studies, 63, 370-382.
Sternberg, R. J. (2006). The nature of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 18(1), 87-
98.
Sternberg, R. J. (1993). Sternberg Triarchic Abilities Test. Unpublished test.
Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1991). An investment theory of creativity and its
development. Human Development, 34, 1-31.
Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1995). Defying the Crowd: Cultivating Creativity in a Culture
of Conformity. Free Press, New York.
Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1996). Investing in creativity. American Psychologist, 51(7),
677-688.
Tadmor, C. T., & Tetlock, P. E. (2006). Biculturalism: A model of the effects of second-
culture exposure on acculturation and integrative complexity. Journal of Cross-
Cultural Psychology, 37, 173-190.
Tadmor, C. T., Tetlock, P. E., & Peng, K. (2009). Acculturation strategies and integrative
complexity: The cognitive implications of biculturalism. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology, 40, 105-139.
Taylor, D. W., Berry, P. C., & Block, C. H. (1958). Does group participation when using
brainstorming facilitate or inhibit creative thinking? Administrative Science Quarterly,
3(1), 23-47.
U.S. Census Bureau. (2000). Current population reports. Series P60-207. Washington, DC: U.S.
Government Printing Office.
LIST OF TABLES
Table
5-1 The means and standard deviations for the ATTA and CCT measures.............................39
page
reliable culture-specific creativity test. Therefore, our findings do not provide conclusive
answers on whether the CCT accurately measured cultural creativity or simply knowledge of
different cultures. Future research is needed to develop a reliable measure of the cognitive
processes that are involved in the creative process, specifically for tasks in the cultural domain.
Measures of cultural creativity should reflect the individual's breadth of knowledge about
different cultures as well as their ability to effectively retrieve and synthesize the cultural
knowledge to generate creative (novel and practical) ideas. Findings from the current studies as
well as previous studies provide evidence that cultural experience enhance creative ability on
tasks that are associated with the cultural domain; however, the measures for assessing this
relationship range widely and are most often broad and subjective in nature. There exists no
measure that explicitly targets cultural creativity, and future research is needed to develop a valid
and reliable method of assessment in this area.
that provide resources such as trainers, teachers, and mentors who effectively guide the
individuals' progress (Erisson & Charness, 1994). Furthermore, the authors' review of the
literature on expertise demonstrate that the mechanisms underlying expert performance, such as
the ability to construct efficient representations of a problem, improved working memory
capacity and encoding strategies, and the ability to anticipate future events, are specific to the
knowledge and skills of the domain that the individual has been deliberately practicing in
(Ericsson & Charness, 1994). The authors contend that creative processes and ideas are produced
after expertise is achieved in any particular domain and, thus, creativity is likely viewed as
domain-specific.
In addition to exploring the relationship between exposure to new cultures and creativity,
this study also investigates the construct of creativity itself. Based on my review of the literature,
the proposed research aims to clarify the nature of creativity as a domain-general or domain-
specific construct, focusing specifically on whether a culture-specific domain exists in creativity.
The Creative Process
Creativity is a process in which a person perceives gaps, contradictions, challenges, or
opportunities and then considers possibilities and different perspectives to develop new and
innovative alternatives (Shoshani & Hazi, 2007). It is most often considered to be a process of
divergent thinking, which starts at a certain point and then branches out in many different
directions. In order to understand how a creative product is achieved, it is important to consider
the factors that are involved in the creative process. Several theories exist, providing varying
explanations of possible elements that are part of the creative process. The following review will
introduce theories addressing the general creative process in regards to the cognitive processes
involved, as well theories addressing the creative process in the cultural domain.
social system, and the individual. Therefore, an individual's creativity occurs within a context,
and there exists a bidirectional relationship between the individual and their context.
All in all, a comprehensive definition of creativity needs to include both the personal
characteristics of an individual and the multitude of variables in the individual's environment
that will influence their creativity. In the present study, I am interested in examining the cultural
component of an individual's environment with respect to creative thinking. Specifically, the
purpose of my study is to develop a better understanding of the relationship between experiences
in new cultures and creative thinking. The following section of my literature review will present
studies that have investigated this link, focusing on the relationship between multicultural
experience and creativity.
Multicultural Experience and Creativity
Multicultural experience is operationalized in terms of the amount of time living abroad,
extensive interactions with foreign cultures, and exposure to a foreign culture (Leung et al.,
2008). Studying abroad provides a unique multicultural experience that involves living in a
foreign country for an extended period of time. This is a quantitatively and qualitatively different
experience when compared to other foreign experiences such as travel (Leung et al., 2008).
Compared to short visits to foreign countries, living abroad provides individuals with various
opportunities to cognitively and behaviorally adapt to a new culture. When living abroad,
individuals are immersed in an unfamiliar environment and challenged to learn and become
familiar with the conventional knowledge and behaviors of a foreign culture (Leung et al., 2008;
Leung & Chiu, 2010) whereas when traveling abroad or visiting shortly, individuals have only a
superficial introduction to the new culture.
One positive cognitive outcome of multicultural experience from studying abroad may be
enhanced creativity. Research investigating the relationship between multicultural experience
Martinez, Chiu, & Morris, 2003; Morris & Fu, 2001). According to the dynamic constructivist
approach, an individual who possesses a large bank of cultural frameworks still needs to be
primed with appropriate stimuli for that information to be accessed and used to its full potential.
Bicultural individuals have been the main focus in the area of study investigating the use of
cultural knowledge and the creative process. The following section of the literature review will
present findings on this body of research.
Cultural Diversity during the Creative Process
Culture provides an individual with a familiar script that embodies their values and beliefs,
as well guides their thoughts and behaviors in a broad range of situations (Lubart, 1990; Tadmor
& Tetlock, 2006). Cultural scripts include but are not limited to physical attributes, eating habits
and familiar foods, clothing, and socioeconomic status. When an individual is exposed to a new
culture, the automatic scripts from their familiar culture that are activated may not be applicable
or even contradictory in the new setting. The individual experiences cognitive dissonance as they
face value conflicts between their old and new cultures (Tadmor & Tetlock, 2006; Tadmor,
Tetlock, & Peng; 2009). Resolving this conflict has shown to lead to cognitive complex abilities,
including the ability to recognize, generate, and synthesize multiple ideas or concepts. One
outcome of cognitive complexity may be enhanced creative thinking, as both are associated with
divergent thinking, perspective taking, and integration of multiple ideas.
As mentioned earlier in my review of the literature on cultural experience and creativity,
studies have shown that cultural experience leads to the development of multiple cultural
frameworks for individuals to employ during the creative process. The following studies provide
evidence that the exposure to and combining of these differing worldviews promotes cognitively
complex abilities. The present study is specifically interested in the cognitive benefits associated
with creative thinking that may result as a possible byproduct of the cognitive complexity among
BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH
Christine S. Lee is a doctoral student in the Department of Educational Psychology at the
University of Florida. Her research interests include the cognitive processes involved in creative
thinking as well as factors that influence creativity, and educational topics related to this field of
study. She received her Bachelor of Arts in Psychology from the University of Florida in 2007.
CHAPTER 3
HYPOTHESES
Based on the literature reviewed, three hypotheses were generated:
In hypothesis 1, I propose that individuals who have studied abroad will produce higher-
quality responses on the Cultural Creativity Task (CCT) (measure of cultural creativity)
compared to individuals who have not studied abroad. Support for the first hypothesis will
provide evidence that cultural experiences from study abroad programs will increase creative
ability in the domain of cultural creativity. Evidence for this hypothesis will support previous
findings that cultural experiences increases creativity in the cultural domain by providing
individuals with ideas from multiple cultural worldviews as well as cognitively complex skills to
employ when engaging in culture-specific tasks.
In hypothesis 2, I propose that individuals who have studied abroad will not produce
higher-quality responses on the Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults (ATTA) (general measure
of creativity) compared to individuals who have not studied abroad. In other words, I propose
that individuals who have studied abroad will not differ significantly from individuals who have
not studied abroad on general measures of creativity which do not have any specified domains.
Evidence for this hypothesis will support findings from previous studies which indicate that
creative benefits from cultural experiences are most pronounced in activities specific to the
cultural domain. Previous studies investigating biculturals that showed cultural experiences
increasing creativity in the cultural domain. Compared to monoculturals, biculturals
demonstrated higher creative abilities on tasks that targeted culturally relevant information;
however, on tasks that were culturally neutral, this creative gap between biculturals and
monoculturals disappeared. Such findings indicate that creative expression is limited to domains
in which individuals have acquired expert knowledge and skills domain (Benet-Martinez et al.,
Ward, C., & Kennedy, A. (1994). Acculturation strategies, psychological adjustment and
sociocultural competence during cross-cultural transitions. International Journal of
Intercultural Relations, 18, 329-343.
Watson, W. E., Kumar, K., & Michaelsen, L. K. (1993). Cultural diversity's impact on
interaction process and performance: Comparing homogeneous and diverse task groups.
The Academy ofManagement Journal, 36(3), 590-602.
Weiner, R. P. (2000). Creativity and Beyond. Cultures, values, and change. New York:
State University of New York Press.
Another aim of my study was to investigate the long-standing debate regarding whether
creativity is a domain-general or domain-specific construct. Drawing from the studies comparing
individuals' abilities on culturally relevant tasks compared to culturally neutral tasks (Benet-
Martinez et al., 2006; Leung et al., 2008; Tadmor & Tetlock, 2006; Tadmor et al., 2009) as well
as the literature on expertise (Ericsson & Charness, 1993), I hypothesized that no significant
differences would exist between the Study Abroad, Plan to Study, and No Study/No Plan groups
on our domain-general measure of creativity assessed by the Abbreviated Torrance Test for
Adults (ATTA). Research investigating individuals who have experienced and acculturated two
different cultures showed that biculturals demonstrate advanced creative abilities on culturally
relevant tasks in comparison to their monocultural counterparts (Benet-Martinez et al., 2006;
Tadmor & Tetlock, 2006; Tadmor et al., 2009). This line of research in addition to the literature
on expertise provide support for expert and creative abilities being specific to the domain in
which individuals' have acquired extensive knowledge and skills (Ericsson & Charness, 1993).
As predicted, my results showed no significant differences among the three groups on the ATTA,
supporting previous claims that creativity is a domain-specific construct. I found that cultural
experience affects creativity the domain of cultural knowledge; however, it does not increase
performance on general creativity tasks. These results suggest that creativity is a
multidimensional construct, and experiences which foster knowledge in a particular domain of
creativity will increase creative abilities in only that corresponding domain.
My findings of the positive link between studying abroad and enhanced cultural creativity
are further strengthened by the results that showed no significant differences between the two
groups that have not studied abroad, the Plan to Study and the No Study/No Plan groups, on both
the ATTA (domain-general measure) and CCT (domain-specific measure). The comparison
the CCT is scored for originality based on a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 to 5: 1 (Not at all),
2 (Little), 3 (.,,i/eii hai), 4 (Much), and 5 (Very much). The sum of the originality scores is then
divided by the sum of the fluency scores to produce a final creativity score for the CCT.
Procedure
Data Collection
As participants arrived to the laboratory at their designated time, they were given a consent
form to read and sign. The experimenter provided each participant with the Abbreviated
Torrance Test for Adults (ATTA). The ATTA was administered according to the instructions in
the ATTA Manual (Goff & Torrance, 2002). The experimenter then distributed the Cultural
Creativity Task to each participant, and it was administered next. The experimenter read the
instructions aloud at the beginning of each activity for both the ATTA and CCT. Each activity
was timed by the experimenter with a stopwatch. At the completion of the experiment, the
participants were given a demographics sheet to complete. All of the forms and tasks were
collected and the participants were thanked for their time. Participants from the research
participants' pool were granted one research credit towards their corresponding course.
Scoring
The ATTA and CCT tasks were coded by one of the authors and a volunteer. Both coders
were both blind to the group which participants belonged to. To ensure that we were following
unitary coding procedures, twenty ATTA and CCT tasks were randomly selected and coded
separately, then compared for consistency.
The Creativity Index (CI) from the ATTA was used to determine whether or not the ratings
were in agreement. In the case of a disagreement where the CI ratings were not the same for a
participant, the total scaled score and the creativity indicators on the ATTA Scoring Sheet of
each rater was examined. We referred to the ATTA Manual together, discussed the differences in
as "the extent to which the ideas could be carried out, given the constraints of reality" (Lamm &
Trommsdorff, 1973, p. 363). The scale for feasibility ranged from 1- definitely infeasible to 5-
definitely feasible (McLeod et al., 1996). Two faculty members in relevant departments, who
were experts in the travel industry, served as judges assessed the ideas. They were instructed to
use the effectiveness and feasibility scales independently (McLeod et al., 1996). The judges were
considered to be in agreement if their ratings of the idea fell within 1 point of each other on the
5-point scales (Diehl & Stroebe, 1987, 1991). The average of each judge's rating was determined
to produce a single rating for each idea, and the mean score of the ratings for all of the ideas was
determined by adding the ratings for each idea and dividing this number by the total number of
ideas produced by the group (McLeod et al., 1996).
The modified version of the Tourism Problem was combined with four prompts developed
by the researchers to create the culture-specific measure of creativity, which we called the
Cultural Creativity Test (CCT). The additional four prompts provide a broader measure of
cultural creativity and take a more comprehensive approach in assessing individuals' ability to
retrieve culturally relevant knowledge and produce novel ideas and solutions. They were
modeled after the verbal activity in the ATTA; however, the content of the brainstorming
prompts were designed to target creativity in the cultural domain. The CCT requires participants
to draw from their knowledge bank of culturally relevant information, including topics such as
waking up with a different skin color, demonstrating high social status, developing new dishes
using exotic ingredients, and creating a product that will have universal appeal.
The CCT is scored for fluency and originality. The fluency score is given for each of the
five prompts in the CCT, representing a tally of the number of ideas produced. The originality
score is a number that represents the degree of novelty an idea exhibits. Each idea produced in
offer students enriching experiences to develop cross-cultural effectiveness and global
understanding, including a greater awareness of one's own country and foreign countries
(Kitsantas & Meyers, 2001). In a study conducted by Carlson and Widaman (1988), 450
questionnaires were sent out to students who studied abroad during their junior year, designed to
assess attitudes towards international understanding (e. g., awareness of problems common to
many nations, desire for international peace) and the degree of change in perspectives (e. g.,
negative feelings towards foreigners, critical views of your own country) prior to and after their
study abroad experiences. Results showed that students who studied abroad during their junior
year reported higher levels of international political concern, cross-cultural interest, and cultural
diversity (Carlson & Widaman, 1988). Furthermore, results indicated that these students
developed a more mature and objective perception of their home country compared to a similar
group of students who stayed on their home campuses (Carlson & Widaman, 1988). Similar
results were found by McCabe (1994) who investigated the changing worldviews of twenty-three
students who participated in a study abroad program at sea. The Semester at Sea program offered
exposure to a variety of cultures, including visits to countries in South America, Africa, and
Asia. The research included in this study was conducted in the Bahamas, Venezuela, Brazil,
Hong Kong, Japan, and the United States. Results indicated that studying abroad contributed to
the development of students' global perspectives by expanding consciousness and knowledge of
the global system, increasing cultural awareness, and developing new sensitivity to other cultures
(McCabe, 1994). Study abroad programs have consistently shown to be successful in providing
the experiences that foster cultural understanding and awareness.
In addition, research supports the benefits of study abroad experiences in multiple
domains of students' lives. Following study abroad experiences, students demonstrate not only
|
Full Text |
xml version 1.0 encoding UTF-8
REPORT xmlns http:www.fcla.edudlsmddaitss xmlns:xsi http:www.w3.org2001XMLSchema-instance xsi:schemaLocation http:www.fcla.edudlsmddaitssdaitssReport.xsd
INGEST IEID EASQNV4EI_R1DZ2Y INGEST_TIME 2012-12-07T22:31:52Z PACKAGE UFE0041870_00001
AGREEMENT_INFO ACCOUNT UF PROJECT UFDC
FILES
PAGE 1
1 CULTURAL EXPERIENCE FROM STUDYING AB ROAD: WILL IT ENHANCE CREATIVE THINKING? By CHRISTINE S. LEE A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DE GREE OF MASTER OF ARTS IN EDUCATION UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA 2010
PAGE 2
2 2010 Christine S. Lee
PAGE 3
3 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This study would not have been possible without the support and encouragement of my family, friends, and colleagues. I also wish to thank my com mittee members, David Therriault and Tracy Linderholm, for their guidance and support.
PAGE 4
4 TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ...............................................................................................................3 LIST OF TABLES ...........................................................................................................................6 ABSTRACT .....................................................................................................................................8 CHAPTER 1 STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM ........................................................................................9 Introduction ...............................................................................................................................9 Benefits of Studying Abroad ....................................................................................................9 Defining Creativity .................................................................................................................11 Multicultural Experience and Creativity ................................................................................13 Creativity as a Domain General or Domain Specific Construct ............................................17 The Creative Process ..............................................................................................................20 Cultural Diversity during the Creative Process ......................................................................22 Cultural Diversity in Groups ...........................................................................................23 Cultural Diversity in Individuals .....................................................................................24 2 PURPOSE OF THE STUDY ..................................................................................................28 3 HYPOTHESES .......................................................................................................................30 4 METHOD ...............................................................................................................................32 Participants .............................................................................................................................32 Measures .................................................................................................................................32 Measure of Domain General Creativity ..........................................................................33 Measure of Domain Specific Creativity ..........................................................................34 Procedure ................................................................................................................................37 Data Collection ................................................................................................................37 Scoring .............................................................................................................................37 Analysis ...........................................................................................................................38 5 RESULTS ...............................................................................................................................39 6 DISCUSSION .........................................................................................................................40 General Discussion .................................................................................................................40 Implications for Cultural Experience and Creative Thinking .................................................42 Limitations and Directions for Future Research .....................................................................44 LIST OF REFERENCES ...............................................................................................................47
PAGE 5
5 BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH .........................................................................................................52
PAGE 6
6 LIST OF TABLES Table page 51 The means and standard deviations for the ATTA and CCT measures. ............................39
PAGE 7
7 LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS ATTA (Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults) Measure of domain general creativity. CCT (Cultural Creativity Test) Measure of domain specific creativity. CI (Creativity Index) Final score of the Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults.
PAGE 8
8 Abstract of Thesis Presented to the Graduate School of the University of Florida in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Arts in Education CULTURAL EXP ERIENCE FROM STUDYING ABROAD: WILL IT ENHANCE CREATIVE THINKING? By Christine Lee August 2010 Chair: David Therriault Major: Educational Psychology There is a growing need for crosscultural competency i n todays diversifying society. C ultural experienc es from study abroad programs have found to be an effective means of providing students with an opportunity to develop valuable skills associated with cultural understanding and awareness. The present study investigated a possible cognitive benefit of studying abroad, focusing on creative thinking. In addition, this study investigated the nature of creativity, aiming to shed some light on the debate in regards to whether creativity is a domain general or domain specific construct. The relationship between cultural experience gained from studying abroad and creative thinking was assessed in a sample of undergraduate and graduate students (n=90) from the University of Florida. The Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults (ATTA) and the Cultural Creativity Test (CCT) were administered and analyzed between three groups: study abroad, plan to study abroad, and no study/no plan. An ANOVA analysis showed that cultural experience predicted enhanced creative thinking only on the CCT, indicating that creativity is a domain specific construct. Finding s from this study indicate that a cognitive benefit gained from studying abroad is the facilitation of creative problem solving abilities in culturally diverse settings.
PAGE 9
9 CHAPTER 1 STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM Introduction Todays s ociety is comprised of a rich blend of ethnically and culturally diverse groups. Currently, approximately 30% of the U.S. population is made up of ethnic minority groups such as African American, Latino/Hispanic, Asian Pacific Islander, and Native American (U.S. Census Bureau, 2000) and this trend towards diversity is on the rise. It is predicted that by the year 2015, ethnic minorities will constitute approximately one third of the United States population, and by the year 2050, students of color will consist of approximately 48 to 50% of the school age population (U.S. Census Bureau, 2000). The increasing diversity in our nation is clearly represented in the growing numbers of individuals from different ethnic and cultural groups within our schools and workplaces, resulting in more and more frequent interpersonal communications among individuals of different backgrounds. As our nation develops into a multicultural demographic, fostering an awareness and understanding of different worldviews is a significan t goal; this goal is especially important for our schools to accomplish in order to produce openminded and informed individuals. Although the need for cultural studies has been recognized for some time, the implementation of effective cultural development curricula has been limited (Miranda, 1999). One area showing movement in students development of cultural awareness is study abroad programs. Benefits of Studying Abroad Study abroad programs provide students with the opportunity to experience a culture different from their own by immersing themselves in a foreign country. Study abroad programs constitute all educational programs that occur in a foreign country outside of the geographical boundaries of the country of origin (Carlson & Widaman, 1988; Kits antas, 2004). Such programs
PAGE 10
10 offer students enriching experiences to develop cross cultural effectiveness and global understanding, including a greater awareness of ones own country and foreign countries (Kitsantas & Meyers, 2001). In a study conducted by Carlson and Widaman (1988), 450 questionnaires were sent out to students who studied abroad during their junior year, designed to assess attitudes towards international understanding (e. g., awareness of problems common to many nations, desire for internat ional peace) and the degree of change in perspectives (e. g., negative feelings towards foreigners, critical views of your own country) prior to and after their study abroad experiences. Results showed that students who studied abroad during their junior year reported higher levels of international political concern, cross cultural interest, and cultural diversity (Carlson & Widaman, 1988). Furthermore, results indicated that these students developed a more mature and objective perception of their home country compared to a similar group of students who stayed on their home campuses (Carlson & Widaman, 1988). Similar results were found by McCabe (1994) who investigated the changing worldviews of twentythree students who participated in a study abroad progra m at sea. The Semester at Sea program offered exposure to a variety of cultures, including visits to countries in South America, Africa, and Asia. The research included in this study was conducted in the Bahamas, Venezuela, Brazil, Hong Kong, Japan, and the United States. Results indicated that studying abroad contributed to the development of students global perspectives by expanding consciousness and knowledge of the global system, increasing cultural awareness, and developing new sensitivity to other cultures (McCabe, 1994). Study abroad programs have consistently shown to be successful in providing the experiences that foster cultural understanding and awareness. In addition, research supports the benefits of study abroad experiences in multiple domains of students lives. Following study abroad experiences, students demonstrate not only
PAGE 11
11 an increased awareness of multicultural issues, but also an improvement in their personal well being including increased self awareness, reflective thought, self relianc e, and self confidence (Kuh & Kaufman, 1984). In a study conducted by Kitsantas (2004), students were assessed before and after their study abroad experience on four dimensions of the Cross Cultural Adaptability Inventory (CCAI) (Kelley & Meyers, 1995). The four dimensions assessed emotional resilience, openness/flexibility, perceptual acuity, and personal autonomy (Kitsantas, 2004). The study found that in the final assessment, students reported higher scores on all four dimensions. Overall, our review of the research on study abroad programs suggests that these programs promote cultural and personal growth in students. However, much of the work on study abroad programs focuses upon the positive affective benefits (e. g., feelings towards foreign countries feelings of personal well being) of studying abroad, and research that explores the possible positive links between study abroad programs and cognitive processes remain largely underdeveloped. This link is important to study because the ability to engage in cognitively complex ways with regards to multicultural information is becoming an increasingly valuable skill to develop in todays diversifying society. Therefore, the purpose of this study is to investigate the benefits of undergoing a study abroad program on cognition, focusing specifically on creative thinking. The growing field of research in creativity is showing that developing the cognitive skills and abilities associated with creative thinking is linked to an enhanced ability to navigate through every day social situations, as well as through academic and professional realms. Defining Creativity Before reviewing the literature on the link between study abroad programs and creativity, it is first necessary to define creativity. Creativity is diff icult to define because it is not an isolated
PAGE 12
12 construct, but one that is influenced by internal, personal as well as external, environmental factors. A widely used definition of creativity is that it is an interaction between ability and process which resu lts in a novel idea or work that is accepted as practical and appropriate by a group of people (Amabile, 1982; Lubart, 1990; Niu & Sternberg, 2002; Plucker & Beghetto, 2004; S ternberg, 2006). Sternberg (1993) argued that creativity encompasses a wide range of valuable skills that contribute to improving students academic performance. Creative individuals engage in decisions that involve redefining problems and exploring alternative perspectives, analyzing ones thoughts, convincing others to adopt ones i deas, overcoming obstacles and taking risks, being patient and tolerant in times of ambiguity, and learning from th eir mistakes ( Sternberg, 1993). He stressed that these cognitive processes associated with creativity are found to be beneficial in the acade mic domain. In addition to being a cognitive process, creativity should be perceived as a cultural phenomenon as wel l. In the Investment Theory of C reativity, Sternberg and Lubart (1991) suggest that creativity can be unders tood as a combination of variabl es including abilities, intellectual processes, knowledge, personality, motivati on, and environmental variables which includes the physical setting and culture of an individual According to this theory, people are buying low and selling high (Sternberg, 2006, p. 87) with respect to ideas. Creative individuals will pursue ideas that are initially unknown or out of favor but then develop them to their potential (Sternberg, 2006). Csikszentmihalyi (1988) proposed a dynamic systems perspective which views the person as part of a system in which social, historical, and cultural factors influence the individuals development and expression of creativity. In Csikszentmihalyis (1988) dynamic system, creativity involves the interaction between three forces: the c ulture, the
PAGE 13
13 social system, and the individual. Therefore, an individuals creativity occurs within a context, and there exists a bidirectional relationship between the individual and their context. All in all, a comprehensive definition of creativity need s to include both the personal characteristics of an individual and the multitude of variables in the individuals environment that will influence their creativity. In the present study, I am interested in examining the cultural component of an individual s environment with respect to creative thinking Spe cifically, the purpose of my study is to develop a better understanding of the relationship between experiences in new cultures and creative thinking. The following section of my literature review will pr esent studies that have investigated this link, focusing on the relationship between multicultural experience and creativity. Multicultural Experience and Creativity Multicultural experience is operationalized in terms of the amount of time living abroad, extensive interactions with foreign cultures, and exposure to a foreign culture (Leung et al., 2008). Studying abroad provides a unique multicultural experience that involves living in a foreign country for an extended period of time. This is a quantitativ ely and qualitatively different experience when compared to other foreign experiences such as travel (Leung et al., 2008). Compared to short visits to foreign countries, living abroad provides individual s with various opportunities to cognitively and behavi orally adapt to a new culture. When living abroad, individuals are immersed in a n unfamiliar en vironment and challenged to learn and become familiar with the conventional knowledge and behaviors of a foreign culture (Leung et al., 2008 ; Leung & Chiu, 2010) whereas when traveling abroad or visiting shortly, individuals have only a superficial introduction to the new culture. One positive cognitive outcome of multicultural experience from studying a broad may be enhanced creativity Research investigating the relationship between multicul tural experience
PAGE 14
14 and creativity show that students who have studied abroad demonstrate increased interest in travel, art, foreign languages, history, and architecture; domains that are all linked to creative pursuits (Carsell o & Creaser, 1976). In a study examining if creative performance results from multicultural experience European American undergraduates who had little knowledge of Chinese culture were shown a 45 minute PowerPoint presentation and then asked to complete a creativity test (Leung et al., 2008) The presentation included pictures, music, and videos based on one of the following five conditions: unicultural Chinese culture condition which consisted of Chinese arts, architectures, food and other elements from C hina; juxtaposition condition which consisted of images from both Chinese and American cultures; fusion condition which consisted of images that depicted the American Chinese fusion culture; the American culture control which consisted of images from the A merican cultures; and the noslide control in which the participants did not watch a presentation. The creativity task required participants to read a summary of the original Cinderella story and rewrite a new version for Turkish children. Two coders rated the creativity tasks on a 7 point Likert scale based on their subjective definition of creativity. The participants in the conditions that exposed them to more than one culture (the American and Chinese cult ures in juxtaposition and the American Chinese f usion conditions ) wrote more creative Cinderella stories compared to participants in the other three conditions who were exposed to either one or no culture (Leung & Chiu, 2010) These findings showed that even one hour of multicultural exposure may engage some cognitive skills that promote creative thinking. In addition, t hese creative benefits were present five to seven days after the original study indicating possible longterm effects of multicultural exposure. Leung & Chiu (2010) also investigated the link between multicultural experience and the tendency to retrieve ideas from diverse cultures and generate unconventional ideas. The Multicultural Experience Survey (MES)
PAGE 15
15 was administered to a group of European American undergraduates to assess their leve l of multicultural experiences. The MES consists of items including whether or not an individual speaks a foreign language, the percentage of an individuals lifetime spent in a foreign country, home country of an individuals parents, the extent of exposure the individual ha s had to other cultures, their five favorite restaurants and the type of cuisine served, their five favorite musicians, and their five favorite friends and their friends nationalities (Leung & Chiu, 2008) The participants were asked t o write down the first five gift ideas that came to mind. Each participant was given dominance/rank ratio score based on how readily the idea was generated by all of the participants. Results showed that the extent of multicultural experiences was negative ly correlated with the average dominance/rank index for the gift ideas. Therefore, a link between greater multicultural experience and the generation of more unconventional ideas was found. Finally, a third study was conducted in which participants were as ked to develop a creative research idea. The participants were given the opportunity to draw from writings of 15 scholars (5 Americans, 5 Turks, and 5 Chinese) who had written on the nature of happiness (Leung & Chiu, 2010). Results showed that individuals with greater multicultural experiences chose a higher percentage of foreign sayings. Based on these findings, the authors suggest that multicultural experience engage cognitive skills such as retrieval and integration of seemingly unrelated ideas that sup port creative thinking (Leung & Chiu, 2010). A qualitative study conducted by Miranda (1999) also found a relationship between cultural experience and enhanced creativity. Four case studies using a naturalistic inquiry method was used to investigat e four individuals experience in different cultures. The participants had spent three or more months in a different culture, and were asked what they perceived were the effects of the cultural experiences on their creative development. A major theme that emerged
PAGE 16
16 from the int erpretations of the data was that the cultural experiences provided the opportunity for the individuals to be more adaptive, innovative, and divergent thinkers (Miranda, 1999). In addition, the participants reported that divergent thinking and risk taking was encouraged in the culture that had a diverse international community (Miranda, 1999). Multicultural experiences have shown to cultivate creativ ity by exposing individuals to situations in which they are faced with foreign cultures that cha llenge them to acquire alternative interpretation s, seek out ideas from diverse sources, and engage in cognitive exploratio n. In a study related to the investigation of the link between cultural experience and creativity, Kharkhurin (2005) found that cultural experience is closely tied to the introduction of a new languag e. Language is inextricable linked to culture, as it shapes thought and guides a pe rsons understanding of the world. It allows people from different cultures to communicate their unique worldviews with each other, affecting each others constructions of reality (Kharkhurin, 2005; Miranda, 1999). The cognitive processes involved in the acquisition of a new language, therefore, overlap with the cognitive processes involved in accommodating a new culture. The simultaneous attainment of two different language systems from two different cultures has shown to impact individuals cognitive abilities that are associated with creativity (Kharkhurin, 2005; Miranda, 1999; Pearl & Lambert, 1962). In a study investigating the possible roles of bilingualism and biculturalism on creativity, Kharkhurin (2005) found that factors involved in learning a new language, including language color terms, language specific descriptors of perso nality, and verbal or fi gural modes of communication have all shown to positively influen ce creativity Two factors in bilinguals development, including linguistic skills and cross cultural experience, were assessed using the Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults (ATTA) composed of three divergent thinking tasks. Findings showed that both factors were
PAGE 17
17 related to fluency, assessed by the ability to rapidly generate ideas and solutions to a problem, and elaboration, assessed by the ability to engage in a detailed analysis of an idea and carry it out (Kharkhurin, 2005). In addition, cross cultural experience was also related to originality, assessed by the amount of novel ideas generated on the divergent thinking tasks ( Kharkhurin, 2005). These findings indicate that the cultural expe rience that accompanies the acquisition of a new language plays a positive role on an individuals creativity. Overall, research has found creative benefits from experiences with new cultures, whether it is through a living abroad experience, seeing image s of different cultures, or learning a new language. Cultural experiences provide cognitively challenging tasks of adapt ing and integrating differing ideas, values, and behaviors from an unfamiliar worldview and have shown to produce creative gains. Creat ivity as a Domain General or Domain Specific Construct Another challenge involved in the endeavor to define creativity revolves around the debate over whether creativity is a domain general or a domain specific construct. In Frames of Mind Howard Gardner (1983, 1999) proposed a theory of multiple intelligence that views creativity as a partially domain specific construct consisting of eight distinct intelligences. Based on his research studying the performance of prodigies and savants, Gardner (1983) conc luded that an innate capacity to rapidly learn material relevant to one of his proposed intelligences existed among exceptionally talented individuals. According to Gardner each intelligence can be relied upon and applied in order to manifest i nto creativ ity However, Gardner also believed that particular intelligences can co nverge to produce creative outcomes (Sternberg, 2005), suggesting that creativity can also be a domaingeneral construct made up of different combinations of his proposed intelligences
PAGE 18
18 Plucker and Beghetto (2004) take a hybrid position on the debate of domain generality and specificity, and argue that both domain specific and domain general features of creativity need to be examined. Drawing from the body of research on general cognit ive abilities and processes as well as those focused on creativity and problem solving (Carroll, 1993; Runco, 1994; Runco & Okuda, 1988), the authors believe that creativity is possible across domains (Plucker & Beghetto, 2004) However, while creative abi lity may be a domain general construct, the interest and commitment that an individual must invest in a particular area is the domain specific aspect of the cr eative process. As an individual gains experience in a particular domain or task, they become mor e domainspecific in their commitment, interest, time, and effort put towards that domain. As a result, the individual is more likely to develop a creative product in that specific domain. In addition, the context in which a product is evaluated for creati vity includes both domain general and domain specific aspects as well. A collective agreement within a society that a product is novel and useful exists; however, there are also contextual boundaries within particular domains that provide guidelines on whe ther a product is creative or not (e. g., assessing a science fair project versus assessing an invention for the Nobel Prize) (Plucker & Beghetto, 2004). The authors conclude that the empirical definition of creativity views it as a domain general construct; however, features of creativity such as the process and evaluation of creativity are domain specific. Other researchers suggest that creativity is a construct including both domain spec ific and domain general features (Amabile, 1996; Sternberg & Lubar t 1995, 1996). In the In vestment Theory of creativity Sternb erg and Lubart (1995) state that creativity consists of multiple components that include abilities, knowledge, thinking styles, personality, motivation, and environmental variables. Each component differs in its degree of domain specificity and domain
PAGE 19
19 generality. For example, the abilities component, including analytic, creative, and practical abilities, can range from domain specific to domaingeneral or sometimes even cut across domains (Sternbe rg, 2005). In contrast, the personality and knowledge components are more domain specific (Sternberg, 2005). In addition, the individuals creative attitudes (i.e. their willingness to redefine difficult obstacles and to analyze solutions to problems), pl ay an important role in their creative ability in both domain specific and domain general aspects ( Sternberg & Lubart, 1995; Sternberg, 2005). There is currently a dearth of information on the topic of whether creativity is a general aptitude or an abilit y that is specific to a particular domain. Research on expertise can be drawn from to shed some light on this matter. Ericsson and Charness (1994) investigated the structure and acquisition of expertise, reviewing the works of influential philosophers and theorists including Gardner (1983), Suzuki (1963, 1981), Young (1759), and Galton (1869/1979) who believed that talent stemmed from inherited abilities. Although this has been the traditionally held view on the nature of exceptional abilities among individuals, empirical evidence for innate talent is lacking (Ericsson & Charness, 1994). In actuality, studies are showing that superior performance in multiple domains including musical, athletic, spatial, mathematical, and physiological activities are due not to innate ability, but rather, the accumulation of skills acquired only through deliberate practice (Ericsson & Charness, 1994). Deliberate practice requires individuals to engage in effortful, individualized structured learning that targets a specific set of knowledge and skills relevant to a particular domain. Studies investigating experts including master chess players, professional athletes and musicians provide evidence that expertise is reached only by engaging in consistent, deliberate practice for a n extended period of time. The development of expertise is also facilitated under optimal environmental conditions
PAGE 20
20 that provide resources such as trainers, teachers, and mentors who effectively guide the individuals progress (Erisson & Charness, 1994). Furthermore, the authors review of the literature on expertise demonstrate that the mechanisms underlying expert performance, such as the ability to construct efficient representations of a problem, improved working memory capacity and encoding strategies, and the ability to anticipate future events, are specific to the knowledge and skills of the domain that the individual has been deliberately practicing in (Ericsson & Charness, 1994). The authors contend that creative processes and ideas are produced afte r expertise is achieved in any particular domain and, thus, creativity is likely viewed as domain specific. In addition to exploring the relationship between exposure to new cultures and creativity, this study also investigates the construct of creativit y itself. Based on my review of the literature the proposed research aims to clarif y the nature of creativity as a domaingeneral or domain specific construct, focusing specifically on whether a culture specific domain exists in creativity. The Creative P rocess Creativity is a process in which a person perceives gaps, contradiction s, challenges, or opportunities and then considers possibilities and different perspectives to develop new and innovative alternatives (Shoshani & Hazi, 2007). It is most often c onsidered to be a process of divergent thinking, which starts at a certain point and then branches out in many different directions. In order to understand how a creative product is achieved, it is important to consider the factors that are involved in the creative process. Several theories exist, providing varying explanations of possible elements that are part of the creative process. The following review will introduce theories addres sing the general creative process in regards to the cognitive processe s involved, as well theories addressing the creative process in the cultural domain.
PAGE 21
21 The creative cognitive approach views the creative process as a result of specific cognitive operations on available knowledge (Rietzschel, Nijstad, & Stroebe, 2007). Acc ording to this view, as long as the individual effectively utilizes knowledge from accessible domains, anyone has the potential to produce creative outcomes (Leung et al., 2008; Rietzschel et al., 2007). The creative process includes a generative process and an explorative process. In the generative process, the individual actively retrieves or seeks out relevant information to generate ideas that have creative potential. In the explorative process, the individual explores and inspects the ideas to determin e which should be further processed. The final ideas are modified, elaborated upon, and transformed to produce a creative end result (Leung et al., 2008). In respect to the present studys focus of investigation, the creative cognitive approach provides an explanation for the positive link between cultural experience and creativity. In contrast to i ndividuals who have experience with only one culture and therefore have o ne highly accessible cultural category individua ls who have adapted another culture hav e two or more hi ghly accessible cultural categories from which they can draw upon during the creative process. Therefore, these individuals have an advantage during the creative process due to the broader ra nge of knowledge they can access from which they can generate and explore ideas The dynamic constructivist approach provides a framework for understanding the creative process specifically in regards to using cultural k nowledge (Chiu, Hong, Morris, & Menon, 2000; Morris & Fu, 2001). This view models cul tural knowledge as a set of different knowledge structures that can be activated by conditions such as priming (Chiu et al., 2000; Morris & Fu, 2001). When an individual encounters culturally relevant stimuli in a particular context, they are able to acces s their cultural knowledge structures to retrieve and apply the information to produce highquality creative products (Hong, & Chiu, 2001; Hong et al., 2001; Hong, Benet -
PAGE 22
22 Martinez, Chiu, & Morris, 2003; Morris & Fu, 2001 ). According to the dynamic construct ivist approach, an individual who possesses a large bank of cultural frameworks still needs to be primed with appropriate stimuli for that information to be accessed and used to its full potential. Bicultural individuals have been the main focus in the are a of study investigating the use of cultural knowledge and the creative process. The following section of the literature review will present findings on this body of research. Cultural Diversity during the Creative Process Culture provides an individual wi th a familiar script that embodies their values and beliefs, as well guides their thoughts and behaviors in a broad range of situations (Lubart, 1990; Tadmor & Tetlock, 20 06). Cultural scripts include but are not limited to physical attributes, eating habi ts and familiar foods, clothing, and socioeconomic status. When an individual is exposed to a new culture, the automatic scripts from their familiar culture that are activated may not be applicable or even contradictory in the new setting. The individual e xperiences cognitive dissonance as they face value conflicts between their old and new cultures (Tadmor & Tetlock, 2006; Tadmor, Tetlock, & Peng; 2009). Resolving this conflict has shown to lead to cognitive complex abilities, including the ability to reco gnize, generate, and synthesize multiple ideas or concepts. One outcome of cognitive complexity may be enhanced creative thinking, as both are associated with divergent thinking, perspective taking, and integration of multiple ideas. As mentioned earlier in my review of the literature on cultural experience and creativity, studies have shown that cultural experience leads to the development of multiple cultural frameworks for individuals to employ during the creative process. The following studies provide evidence that the exposure to and combining of these differing wor ldviews promotes cognitively complex abilities The present study is specifically interested in the cognitive benefits associated with creative thinking that may result as a possible byproduct of the cognitive complexity among
PAGE 23
23 individuals who have adapted more than one cultural framework. Also related to the present study, the following section will provide evidence that a culture specific domain of creativity exist, supporting earlier theori es that suggest that creativity is a multidimensional construct. Cultural Diversity in Groups Particularly in the field o f management, research on cultural diversity in group work indicates that there is value in having culturally diverse individuals work together on problem solving tasks. A study by Watson, Kumar, and Michaelsen (1993) explored the effects of high cultural diversity on group interaction and group problem solving. Participants consisted of undergraduate students enrolled in a management course who wer e randomly assigned to either a group in which all members were of the same nationality and ethnic background (homogeneous) or a group in which at least two members were of different nationality and three members of a different ethnic backgrou nd (heterogeneous) (Watson et al., 1993). The groups were given case studies over a 17 week period and required to analyze these real life company situations (Watson et al., 1993). The case study analyses were evaluated on four criteria: the range of persp ectives, the number of problems identified, alternatives generated, and the quality of the solutions (Watson et al., 1993). The results showed that at the end of the 17 weeks, the heterogeneous group were more effective on identifying problems, seeing diff erent perspectives, and generating multiple alternatives (Watson et al., 1993). A follow up study conducted by McLeod et al. (1996) investigated the difference in performance on a brainstorming task between all AngloAmerican (homogenous) groups and ethni cally diverse (heterogeneous) groups consisting of Anglo, Asian, African, and Hispanic Americans. Undergraduate and graduate students from different academic majors were selected. All Anglo American participants were randomly assigned to either the homogenous or heterogeneous groups, and participants of other ethnicities were randomly assigned to a heterogeneous group. The groups were given a brainstorming task
PAGE 24
24 called the Tourism Problem (Taylor, Berry, & Block, 1958) in which participants were required to formulate as many ideas as possible on how to attract more tourists to the United States. Responses were rated in terms of their feasibility and effectiveness. In line with Watsons et al.s (1993) findings, results showed that ethnically heterogeneous groups produced higher quality creative ideas that were significantly more feasible and more effective ideas compared to ethnically homogeneous groups (McLeod et al., 1996). One explanation for this difference was that the Tourism Problem is a culturespecific task in which the ethnically heterogeneous group would have an advantage over the ethnically homogeneous group due to the diverse range of cultural knowledge present in former group. Cultural Diversity in Individuals Research also supports the creative benefits of cultural diversity within an individual. Studies investigating the creative benefits of cultural experience have compared bicultural s to monoculturals Biculturals are individuals who have internalized two cultures, whereas monoculturals have i nternalized a single culture (Benet Martinez, Lee, & Leu, 2006). Research on biculturals reveals that being exposed to a culture different from ones own facilitates the development of cognitively complex skills that support the creative process. Empirical studies examining bicultural individuals have found that creativity is found in relatively high rates among individuals who are first or second generation immigrants and for immigrants who are ethnically diverse (Lambert, Tucker, & dAnglejan, 1973; Simonton, 1997, 1999). In a study by Tadmor et al., (2009) th e acculturation process and effects of second culture exposure on individuals integrative complexity was investigated Within the cultural domain, integrative complexity was defined as the acknowledgement of differing cultural perspectives on the same issue (differentiation), and the ability to integrate the two perspectives by finding connections between two seemingly contrasting worldviews (integration) (Tadmor et al., 2009).
PAGE 25
25 B icultura ls who adapt ed two cu ltural worldviews were compared to individuals who had abandoned their old culture and assimilated the new culture (assimilated individuals) and to individuals who separated from the new culture and maintained their old culture (separated individuals). Participants included 75 East Asian undergraduate students. Ward and Kennedys (1994) Acculturation Index (AI) was administered to determine if the participants were going to be in the bicultural, the assimilated, or the separated group. Integrative complexity was measured by openended questions which were analyzed to determine the degree of differentiation and integration on a 7point scale. Two questions assessed cultural complexity (e. g., What does it mean to you to be bicultural? Would you def ine yourself as a bicultural?) and two questions assessed general complexity (e. g., Some people feel that organizations waste too much time listening to different points of view and opinions during group meetings. Others feel they dont spend enough tim e. How do you feel? What do you think should be the right balance? (Tadmor et al., 2009, p. 112). Results supported the benefits of biculturalism, showing that biculturals were more integratively complex compared to assimilated and separated individuals ( Tadmor et al., 2009). In addition, integrative complexity among biculturals was greater for questions of cultural complexity compared to questions of general complexity, indicating that level of integrative complexity may d epend on the domain of the task ( Tadmor et al., 2009). Tadmor et al. (2009) concluded that biculturals generally demonstrate greater cognitive complexity compared to monoculturals ; however, this difference is most pronounced when the task is specific to the cultural domain Benet Martine z et al. (2006) proposed that biculturals think about culture in more complex ways than monoculturals, because they are forced to navigate between two cultural frameworks, a process known as cultural frame switching Cognitive complexity is measured by the degree of
PAGE 26
26 differentiation, articulation, and abstraction on a given task (Benet Martinez et al., 2006). It is suggested that the hi gher creative ability amon g biculturals derive from the ir increased flexible approach due to this dual perspective. The hypothesis was tested by presenting the participants with either cultural or neutral pictures and having them write a description of the pictures. The study found that Chinese American biculturals free descriptions of both American and Chinese cultures are hi gher in cognitive complexity than that of AngloAmerican monoculturals. Findings suggest that cultural frame switching may trigger complex cognitive processes that include detect ing, processing, and organizing diverse cultural meanings (Benet Martinez et al., 2006). Benet Martinez et al. (2006) also proposed that the creative superiority (measured by cognitive complexity) exhibited by biculturals are limited to tasks that contain cultural schemas and cues. This proposition is in line with Tadmor et. als (2009) study mentioned earlier, which showed that biculturals exhibited greater integrative complexity on tasks of cultural complexity over tasks of general complexity. It was argued that in culturally neutral domains where explicit cultural references are not present, biculturals will not exhibit increased cognitive complexity compared to monoculturals (Benet Martinez et al., 2006). Findings from their study supported this hypothesis. Chinese American biculturals representations of culture (Chinese or Amer ican) were more cognitively complex (higher in density and abstractness) than those of monoculturals largely specific to the cultural domain (BenetMartinez et al., 2006). Benet Martinez et al. (2006) argue that biculturals superiority on cognitive comple xity tasks are present only on tasks that allow them to draw from their knowledge bank of multiple cultural representations, and absent on tasks outside of the cultural domain. Based the literature reviewed on cultural diversity within groups and individuals, there is considerable evide nce that cultural diversity is positively linked to creativity Exposure to
PAGE 27
27 unfamili ar cultures with differing beliefs, attitudes, and values has shown to lead to the development of multiple conceptua l frameworks which provi des individual s with a rich bank of cultural information. This allows individuals to sample from a broad range of ideas during the creative process. In addition, the process of integrating and synthesizing multiple cultural worldviews has been linked to cognitive complexity. Cognitively complex abilities faci litate the generation and innovative use of id eas during the creative proce ss which results in highly creative products Finally, studies on biculturals show that the cognitive complexity acquired from their experiences with multiple cultures are limited to tasks in the cultural domain.
PAGE 28
28 CHAPTER 2 P URPOSE OF THE STUDY The existing literature on study abroad programs provides substantial evidence that a multitude of benefits exist from the cultural experi ences gained while living abroad. Individuals who ha ve studied abroad demonstrate increased development in cultural understanding and awareness as well as higher levels of overall personal well being (Carlson & Widaman, 1988; Kitsantas, 2004; Kuh & Kaufman, 1984; McCabe 1994). The purpose of this study is to pursue another possible bene fit from study abroad programs: creativity. The liter ature reviewed provides evidence for the relationship between cultural experience and enhanced creativity. In the past, t his relationship has been studied by manipulating cultural experience through the use of cultural images (Benet Martinez et al., 2006; Leung et. al, 2008), conducting qualitative case studies of individuals who have studied abroad (Miranda, 1999), and exam ining the relationship between bilingualism and creativity (Kharkhurin, 2005). The aim of the present study is to contribute to this area of research by integrating the findings on study abroad programs as well as the findings on cultural experience and cr eativity by conducting an empirical study using study abroad experiences as the criteria for having cultural experience to investigate the possible influence it may have on creativity. Significance for theory: Also related to the present study is the research on biculturals that show a relationship between experiencing and adapting a new culture and increased cognitive complexity (Benet Martinez et al., 2006; Tadmor & Tetlock, 2006; Tadmor et al., 2009). I am interested in this line of work due to the simi lar cognitive processes that exist in cognitively complex tasks and creative tasks. However, these studies have been limited to studying biculturals who are of another ethnicity, primarily first or second generation Asian Americans. The present study aims to extend these findings to a broader population by examining the
PAGE 29
29 relationship between cultural experience and creativity among any individuals who have adapted a new cultural worldview through the study abroad experience. Therefore, this study will include individuals who are biculturals not necessarily because of their ethnic background, but instead due to their cultural experiences from studying abroad. Drawing from the study abroad and multicultural fields of research, it is assumed that the cultural ex perience gained from living abroad develops biculturalism among students as they immerse themselves in a foreign country and adapt the values, attitudes, and behaviors from that new cultural worldview. Finally, the present study aims to contribute to the ongoing debate concerning the nature of creativity. Some researchers argue that creativity is a domain general construct (Gardner, 1983, 1999; Plucker and Beghetto, 2004), while others argue that is domainspecific (Benet Martinez et al., 2006, Leung et al. 2008). There are also researchers that argue creativity can be both domain general and domain specific (Amabile, 1996; Sternberg & Lubart, 1995, 1996; Tadmor & Tetlock, 2006; Tadmor et al., 2009). Recent empirical studies conducted on biculturals point to creativity being a domain specific construct, specifically in regards to the cultural domain (Benet Martinez et al., 2006; Leung et al., 2008; Tadmor & Tetlock, 2006; Tadmor et al., 2009). Research in the area of expertise also point to the abilities asso ciated with creativity being domain specific in nature (Ericsson & Charness, 1994). The present study aims to provide further clarification on the nature of creativity in regards to whether it is a domain general of domain specific construct.
PAGE 30
30 CHAPTER 3 HYPOTHESES Based o n the literature reviewed, three hypotheses were generated : In hypothesis 1, I propose that individuals who have studied abroad will produce higher quality responses on the Cultural Creativity Task (CCT) (measure of cultural creativity ) com pared to individuals who have not studied abroad. Support for the first hypothesis will provide evidence that cultural experiences from study abroad programs will increase creative ability in the domain of cultural creativity. Evidence for this hypothesis will support previous findings that cultural experiences increases creativity in the cultural domain by providing individuals with ideas from multiple cultural worldviews as well as cognitively complex skills to employ when engaging in culture specific tasks. In hypothesis 2, I propose that individuals who have studied abroad will not produce higher quality responses on the Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults (ATTA) (general measure of creativity) compared to individuals who have not studied abroad. In other words, I propose that individuals who have studied abroad will not differ significantly from individuals who have not studied abroad on general measures of creativity which do not have any specified domains. Evidence for t his hypothesis will support fin dings from previous studies which indicate that creative benefits from cultural experiences are most pronounced in activities specific to the cultural domain. Previous studies investigating biculturals that showed cultural experiences increasing creativity in the cultural domain. Compared to monoculturals, biculturals demonstrated higher creative abilities on tasks that targeted culturally relevant information; however, on tasks that were culturally neutral, this creative gap between biculturals and monocul turals disappeared. Such findings indicate that creative expression is limited to domains in which individuals have acquired expert knowledge and skills domain (Benet Martinez et al.,
PAGE 31
31 2006; Tadmor & Tetlock, 2006; Tadmor et al., 2009). Empirical research on experts from varying domains revealed that the knowledge and skills acquired to demonstrate mastery was limited to the domain in which the individuals had extensive practice and training (Ericsson & Charness, 1994). Evidenc e for this hypothesis will suggest that creativity is a domainspecific construct ; that individuals creative abilities depend on the degree of relevant experience and knowledge they have accumulated for that specific domain. In hypothesis 3, I propose that individuals who have not yet studied abroad but are planning to study abroad will not differ on measures of cultural or general creativity compared to individuals who have not and are also not planning to study abroad. Evidence for this hypothesis will strengthen my predicted findings that cultural experience is the variable underlying in creased cultural creativity. This hypothesis accounts for the possibility that initial differences in cultural creativity may exist between the individuals who plan to study abroad and the individuals who do not plan to study abroad.
PAGE 32
32 CHAPTER 4 METHOD Participants A total of 90 students from a large southeastern university w ere recruited for this study. The students were recruited from the universitys International Center by means of the study abroad listserv as well as from a research participants pool. Participants consist ed of 30 students who have studied abroad (Study Abroad group), 30 students who have not studied abroad but are planning on studying abroad in the future (Plan to Study group), and 30 students who have not studied abroad and are not planning on studying abroad in the future (No Study/No Plan group) The Study Abroad group participants (5 male and 25 female) were between 18 to 35 years of age ( M = 20.27, SD = 3.11), and consisted of 12 Caucasian/White, 3 African American, 5 Latino/Hispanic, 8 Asian American, and 2 Other students. The Plan to Study group participants (5 male and 25 female) were between 18 to 21 years of age ( M = 19.03, SD = .67), and consisted of 19 Caucasian/White, 5 African American, 4 Latino/Hispanic, and 2 Other students. The No Study/No Plan group participants (5 male and 25 female) were between 18 to 22 years of age ( M = 19.76, SD = 1.64), and consisted of 21 Caucasian/White, 4 African American, 3 Latino/Hispani c, and 2 Other students. Measures Based on the hypotheses proposed, two measures of creativity will be employed. The Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults (ATTA; Goff & Torrance, 2002), was administered to a ssess domain general creativity, and The Cultural Creativity Test was administered to assess domain specific creativity in the cultural domain.
PAGE 33
33 Measure of Domain General Creativity Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults (ATTA; Goff & Torrance, 2002). The Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking (TTCT; Torrance, 1966) is the standard measure for assessing creative abilities The TTCT requires about 45 minutes for the Verbal tests, and 30 minutes for the Figural tests (TTCT; Torrance, 1966) The Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults (ATTA; Goff & Torrance, 2002) is a converted form of the Demonstration Form of the Torrance Tests (D TTCT; Torrance, Wu, & Ando, 1980). The D TTCT consists of activities utilizing the same rationale as activities in the original TTCT, but in abbreviated form requiring considerably less t esting time. The ATTA is used to assess participants divergent thinking abilities. The ATTA contains three 3 minute tasks scored based on four norm referenced measures ( fluency, originality, elaboration, and flexibility) and 15 criterion referenced indicators (e. g., richness and Colorfulness of Imagery, Openness: Resistance to Premature Closure) (TTCT; Torrance, 1966). The total scores from the norm referenced measures and criterion referenced indicators combine to yield a C reativity I ndex (CI). The ATTA begins with a wr itten instruction that explains the general guidelines of the task and encourage participants to use their imagination and thinking abilities. In the verbal component of the task (Activity 1) participants are asked to suppose that they coul d walk on air or fly without being in an airplane or similar vehicle, and then to identity the troubles they might encounter. This activity provides verbal fluency and originality norm referenced scores and five verbal criterion referenced responses. In the picture completion task (Activity 2), the participants are presented with two incomplete figures and asked to draw pictures with the figures and make the pictures as unus ual as possible This activity provides figural fluency, originality, and elaboratio n norm referenced scores. In the picture construction task (Activity 3) the participants were presented with a group of nine identical isosceles triangles arranged in a 3 x 3 matrix and
PAGE 34
34 were asked to draw as many pictures or objects as they can using the t riangles This activity provides figural, fluency, originality, elaboration, and flexibility scores. Activiti es 2 and 3 provide 10 figural criterion referenced responses. Each of the raw scores from the norm referenced measures is converted to a normalized scaled score which are summed to produce a total scaled score. The criterion referenced creativity indicators are scored on a three point scale of 0 to 2. The sum of the total scaled score and creative indicators yield a Creativity Index (CI) (Goff & Torr ance, 2002) The Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults has been reviewed and found to be reliable and valid measures of creative thinking (Chase, 1985). A meta analysis of creative thinking included only studies that used the ATTA in order to insure a vali d and reliable measure (Rose & Lin, 1984). The ATTA has been normalized. The norms reported in the ATTA manual are based upon adults who had completed the D TTCT prior to the year 2000. The manual reports the Kuder Richardson (KR21) reliability coefficient for the total raw score for the four creative abilities measured by the ATTA as .84, and for the total raw score with the creativity indicators score as .90 (Goff & Torrance, 2000b). Inter rater reliabilities range from .95 to .99. Measure of Domain Speci fic Creativity The Cultural Creativity Task (CCT): The CCT consists of five 3 minute brainstorming prompts intended to target creativity in the cultural domain. Each prompt is located at the top of an 8.5 x 11 piece of paper Participants are given three minutes for each prompt, and are asked to come up with as many ideas as possible for each of the five prompts in the CCT. The first prompt included in the CCT is an altered version of The Tourism Problem (Taylor, Berry, & Block, 1958). The Tourism Problem is a task developed for a brainstorming activity. It consists of the following statement:
PAGE 35
35 Each year a great many American tourists go to Europe to visit. But now suppose that our country wanted to get many more Europeans to come to America during their va cations. What steps can you suggest that would get many more Europeans to come to this country as tourists? (Taylor, Berry, & Block, 1958, p.28). For the purposes of this study, the statement was altered: Each year a great many American tourists go to foreign countries to visit. But now suppose that our country wanted to get many more people from foreign countries to come to America during their vacations. What steps can you suggest that would get many more foreign people to come to this country as tourists ? This task is relevant to cultural creativity because of the international focus of the task. In the original Tourism Problem task, t he participants are required to spend 10 minutes to generate as many ideas as they can on 8.5 x 11 paper. Taylor, Berry, and Block (1958) pre tested a total of about twenty problems with Yale students both individually and in small groups. From the pre tests, three problems (including the Tourism Problem) were selected based on findings that showed these tests were most con ducive for brainstorming, seemed to be of most interest to the participants, and produced many and varied responses. McLeod, Lobel, and Cox Jr. (1996) employed the Tourism Problem to study the relationship between ethnic diversity and creativity in small g roups. The Tourism problem was administered to each small group, and the participants were given 15 minutes to individually produce as many ideas as they could to get more tourists to visit the United States 8.5 x 11 paper (McLeod et al., 1996). Each idea was then transferred to an index card with a group identification code on the reverse side (McLeod et al., 1996). Redundant responses were eliminated for each groups set of ideas. Each idea was then judged for effectiveness and feasibility based on a 5 point scale. Effectiveness was defined as how much of a contribution the idea make toward the objective of getting more tourists to visit the U .S (Lamm & Trommsdorff, 1973, p. 363). The scale for effectiveness ranged from 1 would attract hardly anyone to 5would attract almost anyone (McLeod et al., 1996). Feasibility was defined
PAGE 36
36 as the extent to which the ideas could be carried out, given the constraints of reality (Lamm & Trommsdorff, 1973, p. 363). The scale for feasibility ranged from 1 definitely infeasible to 5 definitely feasible (McLeod et al., 1996). Two faculty members in relevant departments who were experts in the travel industry served as judges assessed the ideas. They were instructed to use the effectiveness and feasibility scales independently (McLeod et al., 1996). The judges were considered to be in agreement if their ratings of the idea fell within 1 point of each other on the 5point scales (Diehl & Stroebe, 1987, 1991). The average of each judges rating was determined to produce a single rating for each idea, and the mean score of the ratings for all of the ideas was determined by adding the ratings for each idea and dividing this number by the total number of ideas produced by the group (McLeod et al., 1996). The modified ve rsion of the Tourism Problem was combined with four prompts developed by the researchers to create the culturespecific measure of creativity, which we called the Cultural Creativity Test (CCT). The additional four prompts provide a broader measure of cult ural creativity and take a more comprehensive approach in assessing individuals ability to retrieve culturally relevant knowledge and produce novel ideas and solutions. They were modeled after the verbal activity in the ATTA; however, the content of the brainstorming prompts were designed to target creativity in the cultural domain. The CCT requires participants to draw from their knowledge bank of culturally relevant information, including topics such as waking up with a different skin color, demonstrating high social status, developing new dishes using exotic ingredients, and creating a product that will have universal appeal The CCT is scored for fluency and originality The fluency score is given for each of the five prompts in the CCT, representin g a tally of the number of ideas produced. The originality score is a number that represents the degree of novelty an idea exhibits. Each idea produced in
PAGE 37
37 the CCT is scored for originality based on a Likert type scale ranging from 1 to 5: 1 ( Not at all ), 2 ( Little ), 3 ( Somewhat ), 4 ( Much), and 5 ( Very much ). The sum of the originality scores is then divided by the sum of the fluency scores to produce a final creativity score for the CCT. Procedure Data Collection As p articipants arrive d to the laboratory a t the ir designated time, they were given a consent form to read and sign. The experimenter provided each participant with the Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults (ATTA) The ATTA was administered according to the instructions in the ATTA Manual (Goff & To rrance, 2002). The experimenter then distributed the Cultural Creativity Task to each participant, and it was administered next. The experimenter read the instructions aloud at the beginning of each activity for both the ATTA and CCT. Each activity was tim ed by the experimenter with a stopwatch. At the completion of the experiment, the participants were given a demographics sheet to complete. All of the forms and tasks were collected and the participants were thanked for their time. Participants from the research participants pool were granted one research credit towards their corresponding course. Scoring The ATTA and CCT tasks were coded by one of the authors and a volunteer. Both coders were both blind to the group which participants belonged to. To ens ure that we were following unitary coding procedures, twenty ATTA and CCT tasks were randomly selected and coded separately, then compared for consistency. The Creativity Index (CI) from the ATTA was used to determine whether or not the ratings were in agr eement. In the case of a disagreement where the CI ratings were not the same for a participant, the total scaled score and the creativity indicators on the ATTA Scoring Sheet of each rater was examined. We referred to the ATTA Manual together, discussed th e differences in
PAGE 38
38 the points assigned for the norm referenced and criterion referenced measures, and reached an agreement. The rest of the ATTA tasks were coded. There was a moderate inter rater reliability coefficient ( r = .76). The standard deviation for the fluency scores of each of the five activities in the CCT was determined for both ratings for each participant. The ratings were considered to be in agreement for scoring fluency if the fluency score for each of the five activities fell within the average of their individual standard deviations for each activity. We discussed and agreed upon the necessary adjustments needed to be made to our coding procedures for originality. The remainder of the tasks was coded. We were considered to be in agreement if our final creativity score (sum of the originality scores divided by the sum of the fluency scores) for the CCT fell within 1 point of each other. The average of the final CCT scores for each participant was used for analysis. There was a strong inter rate r reliability coefficient (r = .89). Once all of ninety ATTA and CCT tasks were coded, we met to discuss any remaining disagreement s for both tasks. A 100% agreement on the ATTA and CCT scores for every participant was reached before the data was analyzed Analysis Two one way between groups ANOVAs were conducted to compare the effect of cultural experience from studying abroad on domaingeneral creativity (Creativity Index scores) and domain specific creativity (Cultural Creativity Test scores). Where an overall omnibus F test was significant, we employed a follow up Tukey post hoc test to identify specific difference between the three groups with respect to our hypotheses. The criterion for significance was set at an alpha level of .05.
PAGE 39
39 CHAPTER 5 RESU LTS The Creativity Index (CI) scores from the Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults (ATTA) were used to assess participants creative abilities in respect to domain general creativity. The first column on Table 1 presents the means and standard deviations o f the CI scores for each of the three groups (Study Abroad, Plan to Study, and No Study/No Plan). Results revealed that no significant differences existed on the CI scores between the three groups; Study Abroad group (M = 4.20, SD = 1.03), Plan to Study Abroad group (M = 3.87, SD = .97), and No Study/No Plan group (M = 3.90, SD = 1.24), F (2, 87) = .85, p > .05 (See Table 5 1) The scores from the Cultural Creativity Task (CCT) were used to assess participants creative abilities in the culture specific do main. The second column of Table 1 presents the means and standard deviations of the CCT scores for each of the three groups. Results revealed that significant differences on the CCT scores existed between the three groups; Study Abroad (M = 3.14, SD = .38), Plan to Study Abroad (M = 2.62, SD = .25), and No Study/No Plan (M = 2.59, SD = .21), F (2, 87) = 34.97, p < .05. To further examine the significant differences between the three groups on the CCT, a Tukey post hoc analysis was performed. The analysis r evealed that the Study Abroad group significantly outperformed the Plan to Study group and the No Study/No Plan group on the CCT, F (2, 87) = 34.97, p < .001. In addition, no significant difference existed between the Plan to Study and No Study/No Plan groups in regards to the CCT scores. T able 5 1. The means and standard deviations for the ATTA and CCT measures. Condition ATTA CCT M SD M SD Study abroad 4.2 0 1.03 3.14 0.38 Plan to study 3.87 .97 2.62* .25 No study/no plan 3.90 1.24 2.59* .38 p < .0 5 Significant pairs
PAGE 40
40 CHAPTER 6 DISCUSSION General Discussion Based on my review of the literature examining cultural experience and creativity, I hypothesized that students who have undergone a study abroad experience (Study Abroad group) will produce s ignificantly higher quality creative ideas on the measure of cultural creativity assessed by the Cultural Creativity Task (CCT) compared to students who have not participated in a study abroad program (including both stude nts who have not studied abroad but pl an to studying abroad; Plan to Study group, and students who have not studied abroa d and do not plan on studying abroad; No Study/No Plan group). My findings showed that s tudents who studied abroad did in fact outperform the students who have not studi ed abroad on the CCT. These significant differences between the Study Abroad group and the No Study/No Plan group are in line with previous studies that showed enhanced creative abilities among students who have been exposed to a culture different from the ir own compared to students who have experienced only one culture (Kharkhurin, 2005; Leung & Chiu, 2008; Leung et. al, 2008, Lubart, 1990; Miranda, 1999). In addition, the significant difference between the Study Abroad group and the Plan to Study group st rengthens my conclusion that cultural experience is the variable which accounts for increased creativity in the cultural domain. It was assumed that individuals in both the Study Abroad and Plan to Study Abroad groups are interested in foreign cultures; ho wever, the results show that the actual experience of immersing oneself in a foreign culture has found to be related to increased cultural creativity while interest in foreign cultures without the cultural experience is not. This is consistent with the lit erature on multiculturalism and creativity which states that cultural experience gained from living abroad is qualitatively different than other cultural experiences such as travel or visits (Leung et al., 2008).
PAGE 41
41 Another aim of my study was to investigate the long standing debate regarding whether creativity is a domain general or domainspecific construct Drawing from the studies comparing individuals abilities on culturally relevant tasks compared to culturally neutral tasks ( Benet Martinez et al., 2006; Leung et al., 2008; Tadmor & Tetlock, 2006; Tadmor et al., 2009) as well as the literature on expertise (Ericsson & Charness, 1993), I hypothesized that no significant differences would exist between the Study Abroad, Plan to Study, and No Study/No Plan groups on our domaingeneral measure of creativity assessed by the Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults (ATTA). Research investigating individuals who have experienced and acculturated two different cultures showed that biculturals demonstrate advanced cr eative abilities on culturally relevant tasks in comparision to their monocultural counterparts (Benet Martinez et al., 2006; Tadmor & Tetlock, 2006; Tadmor et al., 2009). This line of research in addition to the literature on expertise provide support for expert and creative abilities being specific to the domain in which individuals have acquired extensive knowledge and skills (Ericsson & Charness, 1993). As predicted, my results showed no significant differences among the three groups on the ATTA, supporting previous claims that creativity is a domain specific construct. I found that cultural experience affects creativity the domain of cultural k nowledge; however, it does not increase performance on general creativity tasks. T hese results suggest that cr eativity is a multidim ensional construct, and experiences which foster knowledge in a particular dom ain of creativity will increase creative abilities in only that corresponding domain. My findings of the positive link between studying abroad and enhanced cultural creativity are further strengthened by the results that showed no significant differences between the two groups that have not studied abroad, the Plan to Study and the No Study/No Plan groups, on both the ATTA (domain general measure) and CCT (d omain specific measure). The comparison
PAGE 42
42 between these two groups was made to account for differences that may have existed among individuals who have not studied abroad. For example, interest in cross cultural experience s was a factor that was present in t he Plan to Study group and abs ent in the No Study/No Plan group, but it did not make a significant difference on the two groups performance on either the general creativity or cultu ral creativity tasks. The lack of significant differences between these tw o groups on both tasks indicates that the cultural experience from study abroad programs is the critical factor for increasing creativity in the cultural domain. The aim of the present study was to focus on the relationship between cultural experience and creativity. The findings from my study provide s further evidence that a positive relationship between the cultural experience gained from study abroad programs and enhanced creativity exists, particularly in respect to the cultural domain of creativity I mplications for Cultural Experience and Creative Thinking There is a growing awareness for the need of cross cultural competency in todays diversifying society. The cultural experiences gained from studying abroad has consistently proven to be an effective means in developing cultural and personal growth in individuals who undergo such experiences (Carlson & Widaman, 1988; Carsello & Greaser, 1976; Kitsantas, 2004; Kitsantas & Meyers, 2001; Kuh & Kauffman, 1984). In the current study, we have found that creativity is yet another benefit from the cultural experi ences gained by studying abroad. Specifically, our study shows that by living abroad, individuals will gain skills that enhance their pe rformance on creativity tasks which require them to employ info rmation from different cultural worldviews to create novel and practical idea s or solution s With the increasing need of mu lticultural aptitude, creative ability in the cultural domain is a n especially valuable skill to acquire. Our study provides support that study abroad programs provide students with the cultural experiences that support creative development in the cultural domain. A significant
PAGE 43
43 benefit of studying abroad seems to be the facilitation creative problem solving abilities in culturally diver se settings. The current study also has broader implications for the benefits of cultural experience on individuals overall cognitive capacities. Studies of bicultural individuals show that the experience of immersing oneself in a foreign culture provides a cognitively complex challenge. Creativity includes many of the skills that are involved in cognitively complex tasks such as differentiation and integration of ideas (Benet Martinez et al., 2006; Tadmor & Tetlock, 2006; Tadmor et al., 2009) Similar to cognitively complex cultural tasks i ndividuals who produce highquality culturally relevant creative ideas engage in the attainment and synthesis of two or more cultural frames (Tadmor et al., 2009), and are able to more efficiently generate and explore relevant information during the creative process (Leung et al., 2008; Rietzschel et al., 2007). In the present study, w e found evidence that these cognitively complex abilities are developed within the culture specific domain in which individuals have gained knowledge and experience during their time abroad. H owever, implications for these findings can be applied to the creative process outside of the cultural domain. To achieve the skills required to effectively utilize informa tion at a cognitively complex level in order to demonstrate creativity, individuals must undergo indepth experience s within specific domains. Th is journey requires a profound and reflective immersion into a specific domain in order to notice and attempt to resolve all of the nuances, exceptions, and contradictory information that may be present. For example, in the present study, individuals who studied abroad were challenged to adapt their culturally familiar script in order to incorporate their new cultural experience that contained foreign beliefs, attitudes, and behaviors. A broad implication of the current studys findings is that the achievement of a creative product in any domain is not possible without first developing a
PAGE 44
44 certain degree of expertise in the area. This is not easi ly achieved, often involving personal challenges, struggles and even resistance from within oneself or others However, overcoming the initial cognitive difficu lties has shown to result in valuable cognitive ski lls and abilities that lead to the production of quality creative ideas Finally, the current study has provided some insight in clarifying the construct of creativity. The nature of creativity has been debated for decades. One of these debates revolves around whether creativity is a domaingeneral or domainspecific construct. Many of the arguments have been theoretical in nature (Amabile, 1996; Gardner, 183, 1999; Sternberg & Lubart, 1995, 1996) ; however, recent research has begun to empirically examine the possible multidimensional nature of crea tivity, focusing on the culture specific domain (Benet Martinez et al., 2006; Tadmor & Tetlock, 2006; Tadmor et al., 2009). In line with pr evious findings, our study suggests that a domain of cultural creativity exists The mounting evidence that creativity is in fact a domain specific construct has implications for the assessment of creativity across all types of creativity tasks. M ultiple measures of creativi ty need to exist, targeting specific domains of knowledge i n order to accurately assess creativity In addition, the findings from the current study imply that any individual has the potential to be creative; however, their creative ability will depend on the e xtent to which they are proficient with a specific domain and will be limited to that domain. Future research is needed to identify additional domains creativity in order to gain a deeper understanding of this construct Limitations and Directions for Future Research Limitations in the current study include the selection of participants. All p ar t icipants in the study were chosen from the University of Florida, including undergraduate and graduate students. Because of the highly educated participant sample, findings may not be generalizeable to the rest of the population. Future research is neede d to include individuals outside of the
PAGE 45
45 college environment that have undergone cultural experience from living abroad so that our findings may be applicable to a broader population. The current study also did not account for the variability in the study abroad experiences that individuals may have undergone. Factors such as the length of the study abroad program, the country in which they lived abroad, and whether or not they lived with a host family were not considered in the current study. Participants fr om the present study reported a wide range of study abroad experiences. Countries in which students studied abroad included Australia, Belgium, Germany, Hong Kong, Paris, Puerto Rico, India, Italy, Japan, London, Mexico, Spain, Thailand, Yucatan Peninsula and a Semester at Sea which included visits to Spain, Italy, Croatia, Greece, Turkey, Bosnia, Bulgaria, Egypt, and Morocco. The length of time spent studying abroad ranged between 3 weeks to 4 months. Research regarding study abroad programs suggest that d epending on the cultural context, creativity can be encouraged or stifled across different domains of creativity (e. g. arts, politics, economics, and religion) ( Oner, 2000; Paletz & Peng, 2008; Rudowicz, 2003; Weiner, 2000). In addition, c ultures may vary in the degree to which divergent thinking versus conformity in thinking is encouraged or condemned (Miranda, 1999; Rudowicz, 2003) Future research is needed to more carefully examine the variability across study abroad experiences and the possible effect s these variables may have on creativity. For example, a possible area for future research is to investigate if there is a linear pattern in the amount of cultural experience and creativity, or if there is a specific amount of time that the individual must spend abroad before creative benefits begin to emerge. There is also a concern with the measure used for assessing cultural creativity. The measure used in the current study (CCT) was a broad assessment for determining whether or not an individual posse ssed cultural creativity. The CCT has not been established as a valid and
PAGE 46
46 reliable culturespecific creativity test. Therefore, our findings do not provide conclusive answers on whether the CCT accurately measured cultural creativity or simply knowledge of different cultures. Future research is needed to develop a reliable measure of the cognitive processes that are involved in the creative process, specifically for tasks in the cultural domain. Measures of cultural creativity should reflect the individual s breadth of knowledge about different cultures as well as their ability to effectively retrieve and synthesize the cultural knowledge to generate creati ve (novel and practical) ideas. Findings from the current studies as well as previous studies provide e vidence that cultural experience enhance creative ability on tasks that are associated with the cultural do main; however, the measures for assessing this relationship range widely and are most oft en broad and subjective in nature There exists no measure t hat explicitly targets cultural creativity, and future research is needed to develop a valid and reliable method of assessment in this area.
PAGE 47
47 LIST OF REFERENCES Amabile, T. M. (1982). Social psychology of creativity: A consensual assessment technique. Jou rnal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43(5), 9971013. Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context: Update to The Social Psychology of Creativity. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Benet Martinez, V., Lee, F., & Leu, J. (2006). Biculturalism and cognitive complexity: Expertise in cultural representations. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 37, 386407. Bloom, B. S. (1985). Generalizations about talent development. In B. S. Bloom (Ed.), Developing talent in young people (pp. 507549). New York: Ballantine Books. Carlson, J.S., & Widaman, K.F. (1988). The effects of study abroad during college on attitudes toward other cultures. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 12 (1), 118. Carsello, C., & Greaser, J. (1976). How college students change during study abroad. College Student Journal, 10, 276278. Chiu C. Y., Hong, Y.Y., Morris, M. W., & Menon, T. (2000). Motivated cultural cognition: The impact of implicit cultural theories on dispositional attribution varies as a function o f need for closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(2) 247259. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1988). Society, culture and person: a system view of creativity. In R.J. STERNBERG (Ed.) The Nature of Creativity Cambridge: Cambridge University Pr ess. Ericsson, K. A., & Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance: Its structure and acquisition. American Psychologist, 49, 725747. Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. Th., & Heizmann, S. (1993). Can we create gifted people? In CIBA Foundation Symposium 178, The origins and development of high ability (pp. 222249). Chichester, England: Wiley. Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of Mind: The Theory of Multiple Intelligences. Basic Books, New York. Gardner, H. (1999). Intelligence Reframed: Multiple Intelligences fo r the 21st Century. Basic Books, New York. Goff, K. & Torrance, E. P. (2000a). Abbreviated Torrance Test for adults Manual Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service. Goff, K. & Torrance, E. P. (2000b). Abbreviated Torrance Test for adults Manual Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service.
PAGE 48
48 Hong, Y.Y., Benet Martinez, V., Chiu, C.Y., & Morris, M. W. (2003). Boundaries of cultural influence: Construct activation as a mechanism for cultural differences in social perception. Journal of Cross Cul tural Psychology, 34, 453464. Hong, Y.Y., & Chiu, C.Y. (2001). Toward a paradigm shift: From cross cultural differences in social cognition to social cognitive mediation of cultural differences. Social Cognition, 19(3) 181196. Hong, Y.Y., Ip, G., Chiu, C.Y., Morris, M. W., & Menon, T. (2001). Cultural identity and dynamic construction of the self: Collective duties and individual rights in Chinese and American cultures. Social Cognition, 19(3) 251268. Kharkhurin, A. V. (2005). On the pos sible relationships between bilingualism, biculturalism and creativity: A cognitive perspective. Ph.D. dissertation, City University of New York, United States, New York. Retrieved May 23, 2009, from Dissertations & Theses: Full Text database. (Publication No. AAT 3169934). Kitsantas, A. (2004). Studying abroad: The role of college student's goals on the development of cross cultural skills and global understanding. College Student Journal, Vol 38(3) 441452. Kitsantas, A. & Meyers, J. (2001, March) Studying abroad: Does it enhance college student crosscultural awareness? Paper presented at the combined annual meeting of the San Diego State University and the U.S. Department of Education Centers for International Business Education and Research San Diego, CA. Kuh, G.K., & Kauffman, N.F. (1984). The impact of study abroad on personal development of college students. Bloomington, IN,: Indiana University School of Education. Lambert, W. E., Tucker, G.R., & dAnglejan, A. (1973). Cognitive and a ttitudinal consequences of bilingual schooling: The St. Lambert project through grade five. Journal of Educational Psychology, 65, 141159. Leung, A. K., & Chiu, C.Y. (2008). Interactive effects of multicultural experiences and openness to experience o n creative potential. Creativity Research Journal, 20(4) 376 382, 2008. Leung, A. K., & Chiu, C.Y. ( 2010). Multicultural experience, idea receptiveness, and creativity. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology XX(X), 119. Leung, A. K., Maddux, W. W., Galinksy, A. D., Chiu, C.Y. (2008). Multicultural experience enhances creativity. American Psychologist, 63(3) 169181.
PAGE 49
49 Lubart T. I. (1990). Creativity and cross cultural variation. International Journal of Psychology, 25, 3959. McCabe, L. T. (1994). The development of a global perspective during participation in semester at sea: A comparative global education program. Educational Review, 46(3) 275286. McLeod, P. L., Lobel, S. A., & Cox, T. H. Jr. (1996). Ethnic diversity and creativity in smal l groups. Small Group Research, 27(2) 248264. Miranda, E. L. M. (1999). Multiple cultural experiences: An exploration of their effects and the fostering of creativity. Ph.D. dissertation, Texas A&M University, United States Texas. Retrieved May 23, 2009, from Dissertations & Theses: Full Text database. (Publication No. AAT 9934461) Morris, M. W., & Fu, H.Y. (2001). How does culture influence conflict resolution? A dynamic constructivist analysis. Social Cognition, 19(3) 324349. Niu, W., & Ste rnberg, R. (2002). Contemporary studies on the concept of creativity: The East and the West. Journal of Creative Behavior, 36(4) 269288. Oner, B. (2000). Innovation and adaptation in a Turkish sample: a preliminary study. The Journal of Psychology, 134, 671676. Paletz, S. B. F., & Peng, K. (2008). Implicit theories of creativity across cultures: Novelty and appropriateness in two product domains. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 39, 286302. Pea r l, E. & Lamber, W. E. (1962). The relation of bi lingualism to intelligence. Psychological Monographs, 76, 23. Plucker, J. A., & Beghetto, R. A. (2004). Why creativity is domain general, why it looks domain specific, and why the distinction does not matter. Creativity: From potential to realization. S ternberg, Robert J. (Ed); Grigorenko, Elena L. (Ed); Singer, Jerome L. (Ed); pp. 153167. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association, 2004. x, 226 pp. Rietzschel, E. F., Nijstad, B. A., & Stroebe, W. (2007). Relative accessibility of domain knowledge and creativity: The effects of knowledge activation on the quantity and originality of generated ideas. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(6) 933946. Rudowicz, E. (2003). Creativity and culture: a two way interaction. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 47(3) 2003.
PAGE 50
50 Shoshani, Y., & Hazi, R. B. (2007). The use of the internet environment for enhancing creativity. Educational Media International, 44(1) 1732. Simonton, D. K. (1997). Foreign influence and national ach ievement: The impact of open milieus on Japanese civilization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 86 94. Simonton, D. K. (1999). Origins of genius: Darwinian perspectives on creativity. New York: Oxford University Press. Sternberg, J. R. (2006). Creating a vision of creativity: The first 25 years. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, S(1) 212. Sternberg, J. R. (2005). Creativity or creativities? International Journal of HumanComputer Studies, 63, 370382. Sternberg, R. J. (2006). The nature of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 18(1) 8798. Sternberg, R. J. (1993). Sternberg Triarchic Abilities Test. Unpublished test. Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1991). An investment theory of creativity and its de velopment. Human Development, 34, 131. Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1995). Defying the Crowd: Cultivating Creativity in a Culture of Conformity. Free Press, New York. Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1996). Investing in creativity. American Psychologist, 51(7) 677688. Tadmor, C. T., & Tetlock, P. E. (2006). Biculturalism: A model of the effects of secondculture exposure on acculturation and integrative complexity. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 37, 173190. Tadmor, C. T., Tetlock, P. E., & Peng, K. (2009). Acculturation strategies and integrative complexity: The cognitive implications of biculturalism. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 40, 105139. Taylor, D. W., Berry, P. C., & Block, C. H. (1958). Does group participati on when using brainstorming facilitate or inhibit creative thinking? Administrative Science Quarterly, 3(1) 2347. U.S. Census Bureau. (2000). Current population reports Series P60 207. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
PAGE 51
51 Ward, C., & Ke nnedy, A. (1994). Acculturation strategies, psychological adjustment and sociocultural competence during cross cultural transitions. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 18 329343. Watson, W. E., Kumar, K., & Michaelsen, L. K. (1993). Cultural diversitys impact on interaction process and performance: Comparing homogeneous and diverse task groups. The Academy of Management Journal, 36(3) 590 602. Weiner, R. P. (2000). Creativity and Beyond. Cultures, values, and change New York: S tate University of New York Press.
PAGE 52
52 BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH Christine S. Lee is a doctoral student in the Department of Educational Psychology at the University of Florida. Her research interests include the cognitive processes involved in creative thinking as well as factors that influence creativity and educational topics related to this field of study. She received her Bachelor of Arts in P sychology from the University of Florida in 2007.
|
|