|
Citation |
- Permanent Link:
- http://ufdc.ufl.edu/UF00102831/00001
Material Information
- Title:
- Imaginative play predisposition, playfulness, ideational fluency
- Creator:
- Depper, Devora Sue
- Copyright Date:
- 1979
- Language:
- English
Subjects
- Subjects / Keywords:
- Child psychology ( jstor )
Childrens games ( jstor ) Conceptualization ( jstor ) Dolls ( jstor ) Humans ( jstor ) Philosophical psychology ( jstor ) Psychology ( jstor ) Spontaneity ( jstor ) Theater ( jstor ) Toys ( jstor )
Record Information
- Source Institution:
- University of Florida
- Holding Location:
- University of Florida
- Rights Management:
- All applicable rights reserved by the source institution and holding location.
- Resource Identifier:
- 05802085 ( OCLC )
AAK5892 ( LTUF ) 0023103529 ( ALEPH )
|
Downloads |
This item has the following downloads:
|
Full Text |
IMAGINATIVE PLAY PREDISPOSITION, PLAYFULNESS,
IDEATIONAL FLUENCY: THEIR RELATIONSHIPS
IN KINDERGARTEN AND FIRST GRADE CHILDREN
BY
DEVORA SUE DEPPER
A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE COUNCIL
OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT
OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY
UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA
1979
Copyright 1979
By
Devora Sue Depper
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This research would not have been possible without the
support and guidance of many individuals. My doctoral com-
mittee provided both guidance in experimental rigor as
well as personal support. Of particular note was my chair
Jacquelin Goldman whose personal investment in my work has
provided a significant enhancement of my professional de-
velopment. In addition the gentle wisdom and loving sup-
port of Benjamen Barger has been unwavering throughout my
graduate training. I would also like to thank the teachers
and administration of P.K. Yonge Laboratory School. With-
out their cooperation this study would not have been pos-
sible. Finally, the immeasurable support and love of my
family and friends enabled me to maintain my own playful-
ness throughout this research.
iii
TABLE OF CONTENTS
Page
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS . . . . .
ABSTRACT . . . . . .
CHAPTER
INTRODUCTION AND HISTORICAL PERSPEC-
TIVE ON THEORIES OF PLAY . .
Classical Theories . . .
Recent Theories . . .
IMAGINATIVE PLAY PREDISPOSITION, PLAY-
FULNESS, AND IDEATIONAL FLUENCY:
THEORETICAL UNDERPINNINGS AND EXPERI-
MENTAL DATA BASE . . .
Relationship of Play to Psychopathology
Antecedents to Imaginative Play .
Relationship of Play Behavior and
Playfulness . . . .
Hypotheses . . . .
METHODS OF PRESENT STUDY
Selection of the Population . .
Subjects . . . .
Procedure . . . .
Means of Evaluation . . .
Validation and Reliability of the
Tests . . . .
Data on Validity from the Literature
Data on Reliability from the
Literature . . . .
The Playfulness Scale . .
The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test
Measuring Imaginative Play Predisposi-
tion . . . .
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION . .
Inter-rater Reliability . .
Hypotheses I and II . . .
Hypothesis III . . .
Hypothesis IV . . . .
Hypothesis V, VI and VII . .
Discussion . . . .
iii
ONE
TWO
2
S 5
16
19
S 20
25
32
THREE
S. 35
S. 35
S. 36
S. 36
S. 38
S. 40
41
S. 41
S. 42
43
. 44
FOUR
50
50
53
56
57
58
59
Page
APPENDICES
IDEATIONAL FLUENCY TASKS:
INTERVIEW SCHEDULE . .
IDEATIONAL FLUENCY TASKS: GUIDE
FOR SCORING . . .
PLAYFULNESS SCALE . .
IMAGINATIVE PLAY PREDISPOSITION
INTERVIEW . . .
HOLTZMAN SCORING GUIDELINES
. 81
S 83
. 95
REFERENCES . . . .
BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH . . . .
103
110
Abstract of Dissertation Presented to the Graduate Council
of the University of Florida in Partial Fulfillment of the
Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy
IMAGINATIVE PLAY PREDISPOSITION, PLAYFULNESS,
IDEATIONAL FLUENCY: THEIR RELATIONSHIPS
IN KINDERGARTEN AND FIRST GRADE CHILDREN
BY
DEVORA SUE DEPPER
JUNE 1979
Chairman: Jacquelin Goldman, Ph.D.
Major Department: Psychology
Children's play behavior has been the focus of numer-
ous experimental studies and theoretical conceptualizations.
As a body of literature, however, it is markedly diverse
and without agreement as to what play behavior is or its
significance. This study examined three constructs: play-
fulness, imaginative play predisposition and ideational flu-
ency which have theoretically encompassed both the affec-
tive and cognitive functions of play behavior. This was
done in an effort to provide a more unified theoretical
conceptualization as well as an experimental data base for
developmental aspects of play behavior. In addition, an
extensive literature review of the theoretical literature
on play behavior was provided.
Seventy-nine kindergarten and first-grade white chil-
dren from five socioeconomic status backgrounds were stud-
ied in a semi-urban school. All children were of at least
normal intelligence and participating in the regular school
program. Playfulness, a multidimensional construct, was
examined utilizing a teacher rating scale developed by
Lieberman. This construct hypothesizes five qualitative
and quantitative dimensions of playfulness: physical spon-
taneity, social spontaneity, humor, manifest joy, and cog-
nitive spontaneity. Imaginative play predisposition, the
ability to interject an "as if" quality into play, was
measured utilizing Singer's interview technique as well
as the Holtzman Inkblot Test. Two different scoring tech-
niques were utilized for the Holtzman Inkblot Test in an
effort to further clarify the methodology necessary to
measure imaginative play predisposition in a younger popu-
lation. Ideational fluency was measured utilizing Lieber-
man's modification of Torrance's work. It was hypothesized
that these three constructs could be measured and repli-
cated on a sample of kindergarten and first grade children
from heterogeneous socioeconomic status backgrounds in a
semi-urban school. It was further hypothesized that there
would be positive interrelationships between these behav-
iors, as measured, providing the beginning of the data base
necessary for a more unitary theoretical conceptualization
of play.
Imaginative play predisposition was replicated on this
population with approximately 50% of the subjects demon-
strating high imaginative play predisposition and 50% dem-
onstrating low imaginative play predisposition. There were
vii
continued difficulties with the Holtzman Inkblot Test, as
an appropriate methodology for a younger population. The
evidence for ideational fluency was replicated on this pop-
ulation. The Playfulness Scale proved problematical in that
the reliability of the data is somewhat questionable. Sig-
nificant relationships were found between imaginative play
predisposition and cognitive spontaneity. The relationship
between imaginative play predisposition as measured by the
Singer interview plus the Holtzman movement score and the
quantitative index for cognitive spontaneity as measured
by the Lieberman rating scale was significant at the .005
level. Imaginative play predisposition as measured by the
Singer interview plus the Holtzman movement score and the
qualitative index for cognitive spontaneity as measured
by the Lieberman rating scale was significant at the .05
level. Imaginative play predisposition as measured by the
Singer interview plus the Holtzman movement plus human re-
sponse and the quantitative index for cognitive spontaneity
as measured by the Lieberman rating scale was related at
the .005 level of significance. Discussion provides clar-
ification of results, new methodological considerations, as
well as new directions for future research.
viii
CHAPTER I
INTRODUCTION AND HISTORICAL
PERSPECTIVE ON THEORIES OF PLAY
Children's play has captured the interest of philos-
ophers, psychologists, anthropologists and sociologists.
As Plato is paraphrased by Erikson, the model of true play-
fulness is
in the need of all young creatures, animal and
human, to leap. To truly leap, you must learn
how to use the ground as a springboard and how
to land resiliently and safely. It means to
test the leeway allowed by given limits; to out-
do and yet not escape gravity. Thus, wherever
playfulness prevails there is always a surpriz-
ing element, surpassing mere repetition or
habituation and at its best suggesting some
virgin chance conquered.(Erikson, 1977, p. 17)
Despite a longstanding interest, a variety of theoretical
conceptualizations and numerous studies, there is little
agreement as to what play is, or the significance of this be-
havior. Weisler and McCall (1976) note that there is an
inflationary trend in the number of articles produced each
year but a recession in the real production of knowledge in
that area. They suggest that part of the difficulty lies
in the lack of a precise definition of play and the lack of
a comprehensive theory of play. This paper will first pro-
vide a historical perspective on the various trends in play
theory. Then two recent conceptualizations of play behavior,
2
Singer's (1968) construct of imaginative play and Lieber-
man's (1964) construct of playfulness will be discussed
in light of their usefulness for providing unity in the
area.
A brief look at various theoretical perspectives of
play is necessary at this point to provide a fuller per-
spective on psychological thinking regarding play behavior.
There have been numerous conceptualizations of play. Gil-
more (1966) divides these theories into two categories.
The classical theories, which are concerned with the ante-
cedents and purposes of play and not its content, comprise
the first group. The more recent theories concerned with
the form of play in relation to specifying its cause and
effects comprise the second group.
Classical Theories
Within the classical theories, Gilmore (1966) notes
that one of the oldest theoretical statements concerning
the significance of play is attributed to both Schiller
(1875) and Spencer (1873). This is the surplus energy
theory. This theory states that because the young are
taken care of by their parents, they have a surplus of
energy, as they do not expend energy for self-preservation.
This energy surplus is released through the exuberant ac-
tivities of play. The theory postulates two things: first,
there is a quantity of energy available to the organism;
and second, that there is a tendency to expend this energy
through play, although it is not necessary for the main-
tenance of a life balance (Gilmore, 1966). This theory
has been put forth by others. Gilmore (1966) notes Terman
(1932), Tinklepaugh (1942), and Alexander (1958).
A second classical theory of play is the relaxation
theory of play (Gilmore, 1966). Play activity is the prod-
uct of a deficit of energy, not a surplus. Lazarus (1883)
and Patrick (1916) are associated with this theory in which
play is seen as the method by which spent energy is replen-
ished (Gilmore, 1966). Gilmore explains that play is a
mode for dissipating the inhibition built up from fatigue
due to tasks that are new to the organism. It follows then
that most play would occur in childhood, as this is a time
for acquiring new skills. Play shows very little buildup
of inhibition because it reflects deep rooted race habits
(psychogenically acquired behaviors that are not new to the
organism) (Gilmore, 1966).
Theorists have also seen play as a form of instinctive
behavior (Gilmore, 1966). Britt and Janus (1941) and Beach
(1945) list approximately two dozen theorists who see play
as a form of instinctive behavior. Karl Groos (1898, 1908)
is one of these theorists whose theory is known as the pre-
exercise theory of play. In this theory play is the product
of emerging instincts. Play is the exercising of the
emerging instincts in preparation for their time of matura-
tion.
G. Stanley Hall (1906), a contemporary of Groos, put
forward his recapitulation theory of play. In this theory
the purpose of play is to rid the organism of unnecessary
instinctual skills which are the legacy of heredity. This
is in striking contrast to Groos' theory; rather than de-
veloping new instinctual skills play is now proposed to
eliminate primitive instinctual skills. Hall also postu-
lated stages of play. He was the first to do this, and
stated that each child passes through stages corresponding
to the cultural stages in the development of races (Gil-
more, 1966). Wundt (1913) was also a well-known recapitu-
lation theorist.
An interesting approach to play with an anthropological
bent came from Appleton (1910). Having contrasted play in
primitive societies and children she suggested that play
is a response to a generalized drive for growth in the or-
ganism. Rather than instinctual pre-exercise as suggested
by Groos, play is the expression of hunger within the or-
ganism for growth to the stage at which the instinct could
operate. Play, therefore, functions, as a facilitator for
skills necessary to the function of adult instincts. Since
the child wants mastery and "knows" that play is the method
to achieve it, he plays. Gilmore (1966) labels Appleton's
theory a growth theory of play.
5
The early 1900's also produced the ego expanding the-
ories of play. K. Lange (1901) and Claraparede (1911,
1934) are considered to be the first proponents of this per-
spective (Gilmore, 1966). Ego in these theories equates
with the reality mapping aspects of cognitive behavior.
Claraparede saw play as an exercising of the ego that
strengthened developing cognitive skills and facilitated
the emergence of new skills. Lange saw play as the process
for ego completion.
Recent Theories
The newer theories of play differ from the old in two
respects. First is the focus on explanations of play based
on dynamics of the individual personality, and the second
is the concern with explaining individual changes in play
behavior (Gilmore, 1966). Gilmore labels these the infan-
tile dynamics theories. Piagetian and psychoanalytic con-
cepts of play are the best known theories in this category.
Lewin (1935) and White (1959, 1964) also fit in this cate-
gory.
Lewin's position is not extensively elaborated. Play
occurs because of the unstructured lifespace of the child.
This results in a discrimination failure between what is
real and what is unreal. It is easy, therefore, for the
child to enter an unreal region where things are changeable
and arbitrary.
Piaget's theory of play is more comprehensive. Play
is a result of the child's cognitive structure. For Piaget,
play is the product of a stage of thinking through which
the child must pass in order to develop from an original
egocentric viewpoint to the adult's viewpoint (Gilmore,
1966). In order to understand Piaget's conceptualization
of play it is necessary to examine the process of cognitive
dynamics. Gilmore (1966) explicates this well. Every human
behavior within his environment has two discriminable as-
pects which are central to Piaget's theory. The first as-
pect is the organism recognizing, categorizing and utilizing
events in terms of previous knowledge. He "bends reality"
to conform to his habits, conventions and preferences. The
second is the individual's response to unique aspects of a
new situation which he incorporates to modify or to adjust
to this "new reality." These two aspects are always pres-
ent; one can, however, predominate over the other. Piaget
suggests that these two aspects of behavior come from dif-
ferent sources, appear at different times and develop at
different rates. It is this dynamic which leads to the
appearance of play in children.
Play is that behavior in which the aspect of adjusting
reality to fit one's concept of reality predominates. The
aspect of accommodating to things as they really are takes
a backseat. As this is an aspect of all behavior, all be-
havior has some play-like aspects. Behaviors are all more
or less playlike, with respect to coping with reality.
Play versus non-play behavior is not a relevant distinction.
Piaget outlines three categories of play: practice
play, symbolic play and "games with rules." Practice play
is evidenced in the infant as the repetitious performance
of any newly acquired ability. This will be performed in
a variety of contexts. All new objects the infant encoun-
ters are made to fit this pattern regardless of their ap-
propriateness. New learning does not take place. In addi-
tion, the infant evidences pleasure with this behavior.
Symbolic play has the characteristic of the child symboliz-
ing a behavior in a play. Gilmore (1966) cites the example
of a child putting a rag to sleep. The rag, treated as
though living, symbolizes to the child that which is salient
to him in the concept of sleep. "Games with rules" develop
later in life and the name is self-explanatory.
Piaget also draws a parallel between play and dream-
ing. As in play, concepts in dreams are modified to fit
existing emotions, often discounting obvious and logical
parameters.
Piaget also examined the development of play in the
child. The newborn infant has only limited reflex abilities
for processing his world cognitively, more specifically for
recognizing and incorporating his experiences or to allow
for uniqueness in his experiences. Postulated, however,
is a tendency in all organisms to make repeated contact
with a novel event. This tendency "forces" in the infant
new knowledge, change in habits and new distinctions re-
garding his environment. The infant becomes able to act
in a play-like manner as he becomes able to act by habit,
thereby reducing the number of unique aspects of a given
experience. The play potential is a given in the nature
of the child and his cognitive structure.
Play behavior is reduced in frequency as a function
of the child's experiences. As the child has more expe-
riences, he learns more improved and rational modes of en-
countering the unfamiliar environment. The child no longer
depends on partially appropriate (play-like) responses to
new situations. Adult mastery is hallmarked by the indi-
vidual's reduced need to mold reality to fit his state of
the moment.
Gilmore summarizes Piaget's position as follows:
Play is the behavior seen whenever there is a
prepondernace of that aspect of all behavior
that involves taking in, molding, and using
things, all in terms of one's current inclina-
tion and habit, without deference to any as-
pects of so behaving that might not "fit" in
some sense. Play can occur only insofar as be-
haviors are sophisticated enough to show dif-
ferentiation between the taking in aspect of
behavior that bends reality to fit the self;
and the self modifying aspects of behavior
that bend the self to fit reality. Play can
occur only insofar as there are many different
modes of thought and action into which reality
may be bent. Thus, it is that the newborn shows
no play, and that until middle childhood more
and more play is seen. Finally, play will not
occur insofar as more adaptive responses become
familiar and can be easily invented when
needed. Thus, it is that play is reduced
in prominence in late childhood.(Gilmore,
1966, p. 318)
Cause and effects of play are sharply distinguished
in Piaget's theory. There are two important products of
play. The first is joy or pleasure or some closely related
state. Play brings with it the "functional pleasure of
use" which is readily apparent in the infant engaged in
practice play. The second product of play is adaptive.
Play facilitates the retention of new abilities. What may
be lost through disuse is maintained as these new abili-
ties get more attention than "reality."
Piaget has also provided a system for categorizing
types of play behavior. Briefly, he has noted two cate-
gories which reduce unpleasantness for the child. The
first category is "compensatory combinations." This be-
havior "improves" reality through distortion to fit more
agreeably with the child's conceptualization. The second
category is "liquidating combinations," in which behavior
is freed from strong affect that initiated the play behav-
ior. An example Piaget notes is of his daughter. After
having been frightened by seeing a dead duck, the child
played at imitating the dead duck and made her dolls see
a dead duck without fear. This source of play is similar
to psychoanalytic conceptualization of play.
A psychoanalytic concept of play was first introduced
by Freud (1908, 1920, 1926) in regard to fantasy and repe-
tition behaviors. Gilmore (1966) states that Freud
thought of play as being closely related to fantasy behav-
ior; in fact he defined play as fantasy woven around real
objects (toys) as contrasted with pure fantasy, which is
daydreaming.
Two classes of wishes were distinguished by Freud,
either of which is necessary for play. The first category
consists of the wishes of a child to be grown up or in a
more fortunate position. The child fantasizes a condition
he wishes to exist, in accordance with his tendency to seek
immediate pleasure. Secondly, play arises from the ten-
dency to repeat any experience which has been too much for
him. In this the child wishes to take an active role in
painful situations he experienced passively. Erikson clar-
ifies this as follows:
Play often proves to be the infantile way of
thinking over difficult experiences and of re-
storing a sense of mastery, comparable to the
way in which we repeat, in ruminations and in
endless talk, in daydreams and in dreams during
sleep, experiences which have been too much for
us. (Erikson, 1959, p. 85)
A "sense of mastery" is the most frequently cited ef-
fect of play in psychoanalytic theory. This mastery feel-
ing is limited to play which reverses a painful experience.
Purely wish fulfillment play is pleasurable through the
reduction of psychic tensions. Waelder (1933) pointed out
that play can circumvent the action of the superego as well
as reality. Play makes it possible to achieve what is phys-
ically or morally impossible.
There have been some modifications of psychoanalytic
theory of play notable by Anna Freud (1936) and Erik Erik-
son (1937, 1940, 1950, 1951, 1959). Anna Freud suggests
that play may serve a defensive purpose as well as promot-
ing active coping behavior. In imitative play where the
imitated object is feared, there is a lessening of the fear
either of the object or what it represents. Erikison empha-
sizes the coping aspects of play (Gilmore, 1966). Erikson
(1950) states, The human animal not only plays most and
longest, it also remains ready to become deadly serious in
the most irrational contexts. Gilmore (1966) notes that
Erikson's concept of play disruption is perhaps his most
important contribution to play theory. Anxiety leads to
play but play can become stressful by mobilizing the anxiety
it is trying to process. This results in an abrupt stop in
play. Recently, Erikson (1977) has expanded play as a
model for understanding the ritualization of human experi-
ence.
In summary, psychoanalytic theory emphasizes the adap-
tive aspects of play behavior motivated by intrapsychic
dynamics of the individual. Freud discussed play behavior
within the framework of wish fulfillment. Here reality is
modified to satisfy the drive to reduce conflict. A. Freud
and Erikson further pointed out the adaptive aspects of mas-
tery in play. Erikson (1950) states that there is mastery
of reality through the creation of "model situations."
White (1959) has addressed mastery derived through play be-
havior which is not based in conflict.
White (1959) proposes that play behavior promotes a
general relationship of effectiveness which the child seeks
to maintain or to establish between himself and the environ-
ment. The control over animate and inanimate objects or
situations, especially those which cannot be affected in
reality, is the goal of "effectance." As the child matures,
the gap between what others do and what the child accom-
plishes is reduced. Play is motivated by the ego-competence
energy which is a drive for effectiveness. Play occurs be-
cause one feels inclined toward such behavior and finds it
naturally satisfying (feeling of efficacy). White states
Young animals and children do not explore be-
cause of a desire to practice useful skills
and prepare for future contingencies. They
play and explore because it is fun, because
there is something inherently satisfying about
it, not because it is going to have a value
in a future time.(White, 1964, p. 34)
Focusing on the more recent conceptualizations of play,
Gilmore's (1966) second category, play is conceptualized as
serving several functions in the child. Cognitive theorists
have emphasized the necessity for play in cognitive
development. Psychoanalytic theorists have focused on
its usefulness for intrapsychic development. White has
suggested that the motivation for and utility of play is
in a more general mastery over his world. Two recent the-
orists have also attempted to explicate the function of
play behavior.
Two new themes in the area of play are Singer's (1973)
conceptualization of imaginative play and Lieberman's
(1964) construct of playfulness. Both of these authors
are addressing common aspects of play behavior. Each has
articulated constructs which provide new information and
clarity regarding more specific aspects of behavior within
the general framework of play. By examining their similar-
ities, it may be possible to provide a more comprehensive
understanding of play behavior.
Singer (1973) defines imaginative play as the intro-
duction of an "as if" dimension to the individual's per-
ception of his experience. This is a modification of the
environment based on experience carried in memory with
early imagery. He notes that various other theorists
(Freud, 1929; Lewin, 1935; Luria, 1932) have noted this
transformation, postulating that this quality is fantasy
in the child and daydreaming in the adult. Singer further
articulates this play behavior as play which requires a
central generation of imagery and involves pretend elements,
i.e. changing of voices and roles, changes in time and
space. This definition of play is consonant with many pre-
viously postulated theoretical conceptualizations of play
(psychoanalytic: A. Freud, 1937; S. Freud, 1958,
1959; Erikson, 1950; Hartmann, 1958; Piaget, 1962; White,
1959, 1960, 1964).
Lieberman (1964, 1977) examines a core of personality
traits which she labels playfulness which can be seen as
an element in play, imagination, and creativity. She sug-
gests that there is a developmental continuity of playful-
ness as a behavior and that playfulness survives play and
becomes a personality trait of the individual. She notes
that playfulness is made up of spontaneity, manifest joy
and a sense of humor. Playfulness, then, can be seen in
the perspective of a qualitative aspect of play. Lieber-
man's work is also consonant with previous play theorists
(Huizinga, 1955; Piaget, 1932; White, 1959, 1960, 1964).
Each of these two theorists, Singer, and Lieberman,
is examining play behavior in an effort to provide a the-
oretical unity incorporating the cognitive and affective
dimensions of play. Singer (1966, 1973a) examines the play
behavior with respect to the child and hypothesizes the im-
plications of this play behavior with respect to personality
development and cognitive processes. Lieberman, on the
other hand, explores the personality trait of playfulness
which is theoretically based around imagination and play be-
havior (Lieberman, 1977). By examining the relationship
15
between these constructs and the cognitive component of
ideational fluency, it is hoped that this author may provide
further codification of our understanding of play behavior
and generate further unity within a comprehensive theory
of play.
CHAPTER II
IMAGINATIVE PLAY PREDISPOSITION, PLAYFULNESS, AND
IDEATIONAL FLUENCY: THEORETICAL UNDERPINNINGS
AND EXPERIMENTAL DATA BASE
The concept of imaginative play as defined by Singer
(1973) provides a unity of various theories of play.
Imaginative play is the ability to inject an "as if" or
make-believe component in play. We can conceptualize this
behavior as serving a variety of functions for the child.
The use of imagination and fantasy provide the child with
a tool to facilitate the organization of his world along
both cognitive and affective dimensions. Psychoanalytic
theorists have commented on the purpose of bending reality
to meet the individual's needs. Cognitive theorists have
articulated the process by which the child bends reality
in order to organize his world cognitively.
Freud discussed fantasy behavior within the framework
of wish fulfillment. Here reality is modified to satisfy
the drive to reduce conflict. A. Freud and Erikson pointed
out the adaptive aspects of mastery in play involving imag-
inative elements. Erikson (1950) points out that there is
a mastery of reality through the creation of "model situa-
tions" (imaginary situations). Hartmann (1958) stated that
fantasy is an autonomous ego function which is developed
without conflict. This expands the psychoanalytic
conceptualization of play and fits well with Singer's con-
ceptualization. Singer's (1966) construct of imaginative
play describes the process necessary to reduce conflict
(Freud) or to gain mastery (A. Freud, Erikson). Modifica-
tion of reality by injecting an "as if" or make-believe com-
ponent enables the child to reduce conflict. Singer (1966)
is defining the mechanism necessary to accomplish the func-
tion of play in psychoanalytic theory; that of bending
reality to reduce conflict.
Piaget's (1962) conceptualization of play as defined
by the process of assimilation neatly supports Singer's
theory. According to Piaget, play is not a behavior per se
but a process. Play is the assimilation of reality to the
ego. Play is distinguishable by a modification, varying
in degree of the conditions of equilibrium between reality
and the ego. The child utilizes assimilation in order to
respond to his environment with the schemata available at
a certain age. The introduction of make-believe elements
aids in the maintenance of his perceptions at equilibrium
or in balance with his world. Singer's (1966) construct of
imaginative play is for the most part equivalent to Piaget's
(1962) construct of symbolic play.
"Symbolic play, then, is only one form of thought linked
to all others by its mechanism, but having as its sole aim
satisfaction of the ego, i.e., individual truth as opposed
to collective and impersonal truth ..." (Piaget, 1962, p. 167).
Millar (1968) interprets aspects of play within a cognitive
framework as well. She hypothesizes that imaginative play
is an aspect of cognitive feedback processes which enable
the human to code and process incoming data. This is also
stated by Schachtel (1959). In examining the repetitive as-
pects of play, he notes that this repetition provides the
opportunity to integrate new information into a limited ex-
perience background. White (1960) expands on this theme of
cognitive mastery into other areas of behavior.
White (1960) places the child's play behavior within
the general developmental framework of striving for compe-
tence. The child utilizes play to expand his own capacities
within the limits of his capacities. This includes social
as well as, cognitive abilities. Tomkins (1962, 1963) ex-
pands the cognitive and affective components of play to note
that it is utilized to organize the child's experience along
both these dimensions. The peak of familiarity of material
yields joy and laughter (Singer, 1966; Tomkins, 1962).
Singer's (1966) construct of imaginative play effective-
ly provides a link between the affective and cognitive aspects
of play behavior described in psychoanalytic and cognitive
theories. It does so by providing the mechanism by which the
affective needs articulated in psychoanalytic theory are ac-
complished and by describing the same mechanism which cog-
nitive theorists have limited to information processing.
As White (1960) expands the function of play behavior into
the realm of competence, taking it beyond conflict reduc-
tion and cognitive processes, we can see that Singer's
(1966) construct neatly provides the appropriate mechanism
here as well. The child's affective development and cog-
nitive development are limited by his age, i.e., he has lim-
ited intrapsychic structure and cognitive schemata for pro-
cessing his world effectively. Imaginative play provides
a mechanism for expanding his competence, given these lim-
itations, to achieve mastery.
Relationship of Play to Psychopathology
Singer expands his theoretical work to postulate as-
pects of personality development. Corrigan (1960) states
that daydreaming can be useful as a coping mechanism. The
development of imagination and daydreaming may be associ-
ated with a pattern of development and personality organiza-
tion. This personality organization is one in which think-
ing is valued and affective control and motor control are
emphasized. Within this personality development Singer
(1973) addresses the issues of pathological development.
He suggests that the high fantasy oriented individual will
develop a more obsessive defense pattern. The low fantasy
individual, on the other hand, will develop a more hyster-
ical style, where defenses are primarily repression and de-
nial.
In essence Singer is proposing that those individuals
who have a high imaginative play predisposition will then
utilize this mechanism defensively to reduce conflict.
Again it becomes clear that Singer is describing an inter-
nal process which describes one form of defense mechanism
according to psychoanalytic theory. What he is proposing
is that what psychoanalytic theory describes in children
as play, wish fulfillment to reduce conflict, in adults
is transformed into an internal obsessive defensive pat-
tern. He goes one step further to propose that individuals
with a high imaginative predisposition evidenced in child-
hood as high frequency imaginative play behavior, are more
likely to utilize this process defensively thereby develop-
ing a more obsessive personality style.
Antecedents to Imaginative Play
Singer's (1973) first study of imaginative play pre-
disposition explored several variables: background vari-
ables of the child and his relationship to his parents,
waiting behavior, creativity and personality characteris-
tics. Singer hypothesized that imaginative play viewed as
a manifestation of cognitive and affective style might well
be associated with a pattern of development and personality
organization in which thinking is valued and affective and
motor control would be more emphasized. Contact with at
least one benign adult and the opportunity to be alone
were seen as necessary to increased imaginative play de-
velopment. In addition, from the perspective of psycho-
pathology, high fantasizers were expected to utilize de-
fenses associated with obsessional characteristics, while
low imaginative play children were expected to exhibit pat-
terns more like those of hysterical personalities with less
self-consciousness and greater use of the mechanisms of de-
nial and repression. Singer examined forty children between
the ages of 6 and 9 years, of middle SES backgrounds and of
somewhat above average intelligence. The results supported
several of the hypotheses and clearly pointed out differ-
ences between high and low imaginative play subjects. High
imaginative play subjects reported greater associations be-
tween their parents and themselves, were able to wait for
longer periods of time, had significantly higher ratings
for creativity in a storytelling task and showed more ob-
sessional defense mechanisms than low imaginative play sub-
jects.
Pulaski (1973) examined the effects of toy structure
on imaginative play. It was hypothesized that minimally
structured materials would stimulate more imaginative play
than highly structured materials. Subjects in this study
were kindergarten, first and second graders. Again this
sample was homogeneous: upper middle class children attend-
ing a private school. The results were quite interesting.
Minimally structured toys, for both low and high imaginative
play subjects yielded a greater variety of themes in play
than highly structured toys. High imaginative play chil-
dren played at a higher fantasy level than low imaginative
play subjects. High imaginative play children also became
more deeply absorbed in their play than did low imaginative
play subjects. High imaginative play children also appeared
to be more pleased and interested in their play than low
imaginative play subjects. In addition, high imaginative
play subjects showed a tendency to integrate more than one
category of toy in their play. High imaginative play sub-
jects also showed greater flexibility in coping with demands
upon their ability to produce fantasy. They also responded
less negatively to interruptions in their play. Overall,
it can be seen that there was a distinct difference between
low and high imaginative play children from equally priv-
ileged situations and with equally high intelligence. High
imaginative play children were more original, creative,
flexible and well integrated.
Biblow (1973) examined drive reduction and mood change
in the control of aggressive responses in high and low imag-
inative play subjects. Examining fifth grade white, middle
class children of average intelligence, he found that high
imaginative play subjects reduced aggression through the
use of fantasy. Biblow concludes that the spectrum of his
data supports that there are distinct differences between
high and low imaginative play subjects, both in behavioral
aggression and in all mood states.
Freyburg (1973) examined the possibility of increas-
ing imaginative play in urban disadvantaged kindergarteners.
Utilizing a public school, she found that after one month
of training, the experimental group improved significantly
in its use of imaginative play, as well as, in the expres-
sion of positive affect and increased concentration in play.
The control group remained unchanged. The changes in the
experimental group continued post-training, and were tested
during two months of post-training observations.
Gottlieb (1973) also explored the modifiability of
imaginative play. Noting the decrease of overt manifesta-
tions of make-believe between the ages of 6 or 7 and 12,
she chose to use two age groups as subjects: 10 to 12 and
12 1/2 to 14 years. She was interested in exploring the
difference in fantasy behavior at the two age levels, and
its modifiability given a high or low fantasy predisposi-
tion. She found that expression of fantasy was increased
for young children with exposure to an adult model. Junior
high school age subjects' responses were more directly re-
lated to their imaginative play predisposition, rather than
modeling effects, i.e., they responded more in terms of
their personality attributes. She concluded that imagin-
ative behavior increases with age and as such it can be
concluded that it is a skill that develops with
differentiation, that there are age trends in ability to
engage in fantasy and in the content of fantasy as well.
The evidence for imaginative play predisposition sup-
ports that there are differences between low and high fan-
tasy predisposed individuals, that there are overt behav-
ioral and personality differences. The behaviors related
to fantasy appear to be modifiable given different environ-
ment variables.
The above mentioned studies explore a variety of di-
rections regarding imaginative play. It is important at
this point to note, how if at all, they contribute to the
theoretical basis of play behavior. Singer's (1973) study
most directly explores theoretical concerns by addressing
the defensive patterns utilized by the high and low imag-
inative play predisposed children. His finding that high
imaginative play predisposed children utilize more obses-
sional defense mechanisms supports the hypothesis that this
imaginative process may be related to the obsessional de-
fense mechanism, which in turn leads to the development of
an obsessional personality style. The other issues he ad-
dresses regarding parent-child relationships, waiting be-
havior and creativity do not directly address theoretical
issues. Rather they provide information regarding behavior
to which imaginative play predisposition may be related.
Pulaski's (1973) study focuses more on description of
imaginative play in response to the stimulus of different
toys. This study describes differential behavioral re-
sponses to play materials rather than directly providing
support for theoretical questions.
Biblow's (1973) study, in contrast to Pulaski's (1973)
work, more directly explored theoretical issues. The
cathartic theory of aggression from a psychoanalytic basis
hypothesizes that fantasy operates to lower aggression.
His data supported that hypothesis by establishing that
high imaginative play subjects utilized fantasy to reduce
aggression. This provides data regarding the utility of
imaginative play as a mechanism to reduce conflict.
Freyburg (1973) and Gottlieb's (1973) works both exam-
ine the modifiability of imaginative play. Again as with
Pulaski's work, these studies provide information regarding
the parameters of imaginative play but little direct support
for theoretical conceptualization. Studies in imaginative
play to date have served two useful and complimentary func-
tions: empirical support for certain aspects of theoret-
ical conceptualizations and description of the parameters
of imaginative play.
Relationship of Play Behavior and Playfulness
Singer (1973) and Lieberman (1977) have both addressed
the tendency toward play in children. Singer (1973) sug-
gests that the tendency to engage in imaginative play, fan-
tasied role shifting or daydreaming may be looked upon as a
particular skill that can be developed in a given child as
a consequence of the interaction between constitutional
capacities with a particular set of early environmental cir-
cumstances that provide encouragement for practice. This
tendency or trait in the individual has been addressed by
Lieberman (1977) in her construct of playfulness.
Lieberman (1964, 1977) examines a core of personality
traits which she labels playfulness. Playfulness can be
seen as an element in play, imagination and creativity.
She postulates that there is a developmental continuity of
playfulness as a behavior and that playfulness survives
play and becomes a personality trait of the individual.
She notes that playfulness is made up of spontaneity, man-
ifest joy and a sense of humor. Playfulness can be seen in
the perspective of a qualitative aspect of play.
The theoretical underpinnings to Lieberman's (1964)
work are less clear cut than Singer's work. Essentially,
Lieberman's work attempts to address qualitative aspects
of behavior rather than a discrete behavioral process that
can be identified as evident or lacking. Playfulness is
descriptive of the content of play behavior, as opposed to
the structure of play, e.g., Singer (1973) imaginative play.
Theoretically it is unclear what function playfulness serves.
Playfulness is an expressive dimension of play behavior
rather than a mechanism of play behavior.
Qualitative aspects of play have received less atten-
tion in the psychological investigation of play. Huizinga
(1955) refers to the play spirit. Joy, fun, pretend, and
nonseriousness are key words in his conceptualization of
play. While the fun element is seen as the part of play
that resists logical analysis, it is at the same time re-
garded as characterizing the essence of play. Perhaps to
the extent that play behavior is more or less playful, i.e.,
has more or less spontaneity, joy and humor in it, it is
more closely related to the quintessence of play.
How an individual child plays in relation to environ-
ment incorporates theoretically the playful component of
play noted by Piaget (1932, 1962) and White (1960, 1964).
This is the enjoyment or pleasurable component of play
noted by Piaget (1962). White (1964) articulates this as
fun, the inherent satisfaction of play behavior. This is
a key aspect of how the clinician looks at play. It is the
deviation from the play spirit which is important.
Moustakas (1955) reports on play therapy with normal chil-
dren. He notes that these children are happy, often singing
and humming, and in their actions they are both more deci-
sive and spontaneous than disturbed children. Hartley,
Frank, and Goldenson (1952) found in nursery-school children
that well-adjusted children played as enthusiastically as
the troubled youngsters, but their delight in toys was
greater.
Developmental psychologists have also referred to the
qualitative aspects of play. For example, Piaget (1962)
distinguishes playful from imitative behavior and describes
it as a process whereby the child incorporates external ob-
jects into his own thought schemata in a joyful manner.
Hunt (1961) finds function pleasure in play as a quality
that frequently accompanies learning as a result of aimless
activity.
Playfulness emerged from observational studies of how
children play (Lieberman, 1977). In an effort to examine
theoretically complex behavior she defined operationally
playfulness as physical, social, and cognitive spontaneity,
manifest joy and sense of humor. Within the framework of
play, it was seen as a quality of play. These operational
definitions of the qualitative aspects of play represent
the first step in providing an adequate empirical investi-
gation of playfulness. It is readily apparent that the
theoretical underpinnings for the construct are more so-
phisticated than the operational definitions based on loose-
ly defined behavioral observations. The playfulness con-
struct is an ambitious attempt to operationalize the ex-
pressive content of play behavior. Both psychoanalytic
theory and cognitive theories have more directly addressed
the functional and structural aspects of play behavior
while alluding to the qualitative or expressive dimensions
of this behavior. What Lieberman (1964) is attempting to
provide is a perspective of the content of play behavior:
social, cognitive and affective. In light of this, how-
ever, this is the best methodological exploration to emerge
out of the theoretical speculation.
Lieberman (1964, 1977) has examined playfulness at
two age levels: kindergarteners and adolescents. The re-
search on kindergarteners investigated the relationship be-
tween playfulness and divergent thinking. Utilizing mid-
dle-class children attending private kindergartens, she
found that there was a unitary trait called playfulness
composed of five dimensions: physical, cognitive, and so-
cial spontaneity, manifest joy and a sense of humor. The
relationship between divergent thinking and playfulness was
not so clear. But a clear relationship between playfulness
and ideational fluency and spontaneous flexibility (two as-
pects of divergent thinking) was found. The strongest re-
lationship was between ideational fluency and playfulness.
The adolescent study looked at high school students,
noting that there is a resurgence of the joyful-spontaneity-
sense of humor syndrome during adolescence and that this be-
havior syndrome is related to cognitive style (Lieberman,
1977). Preliminary results yield a more complex picture
for adolescents than kindergarteners. It is not clear
whether or not there is a "pure" playfulness factor for
adolescents. The research was conducted in a classroom be-
havior of intellectual commitment which is not consonant
with the bubbling effervescence of playfulness. Some pre-
liminary work is currently being done on a college age
population.
From the work of Singer (1973) and others (Biblow,
1973; Freyburg, 1973; Gottlieb, 1973; Pulaski, 1973) there
appears to be evidence for high and low imaginative play
predisposed individuals. In addition, the work of Lieber-
man (1964) provides evidence for a playfulness trait in
children. In order to further describe these behavioral
traits it is necessary to expand the existing data base,
i.e., to replicate the findings on a broader data base as
well as explore the relationships between these traits.
The present study explores the relationships between
imaginative play, playfulness, and ideational fluency.
Ideational fluency was chosen as an effort to replicate
Lieberman's (1964) findings and to provide further data as
to the structure of the cognitive aspect of imaginative
play.
Qualities frequently noted in children's play are its
imaginative scope and creative power (Lieberman, 1964).
There has been, however, little research done on young chil-
dren in regard to this creativity. In their initial work on
creative thinking in adults, Guilford et al. (1951) hypoth-
esized eight abilities that might "discover the individual
who is potentially creative." Among these eight was idea-
tional fluency. This is the calling up of ideas in
situations demanding relatively little restriction, the
difference in scores being more a function of the number
of ideas produced than the quality, degree of appropriate-
ness, or the aptness of expression of the ideas (Guilford,
et al., 1956).
While Guilford was refining his factors looking at
adults, Torrance (1960) and his associates used his con-
ceptual framework and an adaptation of his tests to exam-
ine creative thinking in children. The Minnesota research
team was able to develop batteries of nonverbal and verbal
creative thinking tasks. One of the constructs which could
be meaningfully scored was ideational fluency. The purpose
behind these studies was to provide a description of and a
means of testing creative potential.
The present study grew out of several considerations.
There exists extensive and sophisticated theoretical formu-
lations regarding play behavior. The empirical support for
these theories is wide ranging, divergent, and methodolog-
ically poorly developed. Rather than develop new constructs
and instruments to measure them, it seemed more useful to
work with currently defined constructs which overlapped
theoretically. Relationships between playfulness and imag-
inative play predisposition would provide information re-
garding both the structure and the content of play behavior.
While recognizing the methodologies as first steps in oper-
ationalizing complex phenomena, working with these
methodologies to explore their utility is an appropriate
investigation. If in fact the constructs address similar
aspects of behavior, as their theoretical bases suggest,
then establishing empirical relationships could provide
new directions for empirical and theoretical work in play.
The evidence for playfulness, imaginative play predispo-
sition and ideational fluency has been limited to homogen-
eous samples: middle class urban children. In an effort
to expand this data base it seemed desirable to replicate
the data on a less homogeneous sample.
Hypotheses
Hypotheses 1 and 2
Based on both psychoanalytic and cognitive theoretical
conceptualizations of play behavior, Singer (1973) has op-
erationally defined imaginative play behavior as an aspect
of play behavior which encompasses the ability to inject an
"as if" component into play. Utilizing the methodology de-
veloped by Singer (1973), as well as the Holtzman Inkblot
Test It is hypothesized that the evidence for high and low
imaginative play predisposed individuals can be replicated
on an heterogeneous SES sample of kindergarten and first
grade children. It is further hypothesized that a more
appropriate scoring technique for the Holtzman Inkblot Test
utilizes Human plus Movement responses.
Hypothesis 3
Lieberman (1964) has operationally defined the con-
struct playfulness to address the qualitative aspects of
play behavior based on observations of children's behavior.
Utilizing the methodology developed by Lieberman (1964),
it is hypothesized that the evidence for a playfulness as-
pect of behavior can be replicated on a SES heterogeneous
sample of kindergarten and first grade children.
Hypothesis 4
Ideational Fluency, developed by Guilford (1951) as
an aspect of creative thinking in adults, was expanded on
by Torrance (1960) to explore creative thinking in chil-
dren. Utilizing Lieberman's (1964) methodology for kinder-
garten children, it is hypothesized that the evidence for
ideational fluency can be replicated on an heterogeneous
SES sample of kindergarten and first grade children.
Hypotheses 5 and 6
Given the theoretical overlap of imaginative play pre-
disposition and playfulness it is hypothesized that these
two constructs are highly related in kindergarten and first
grade children. High imaginative play predisposed children
likewise should demonstrate behavior rich in the quality
dimension of playfulness. It is further hypothesized that
the subcomponent of cognitive spontaneity in playfulness
will have the greatest relationship to imaginative play pre-
disposition, as it most directly examines imagination.
34
Hypothesis 7
In addition, it is hypothesized that ideational flu-
ency is related to both playfulness and imaginative play
predisposition in this sample of kindergarten and first
grade children.
CHAPTER III
METHODS OF PRESENT STUDY
Selection of the Population
The kindergarten-first grade classes of the P.K. Yonge
Laboratory School of the University of Florida were selected
for the study. Due to its relationship with the University
of Florida the students are accustomed to a variety of in-
terruptions in their normal school day whether it be for
experimental purposes or special observers or visitors. It
was felt, therefore, that there would be fewer effects in-
troduced by the experimental intervention per se as this
is seen as "normal" in this school. In addition, as the
school selects its population to reflect five SES levels of
the community and a heterogeneous population was desirable,
the population was ideal.
There were four combined kindergarten-first grade
classes. Arrangements were made with each of the four
teachers to gain her cooperation and interest in the study.
Essentially all the Caucasian students were utilized to
provide a balanced sample for sex and SES level.
Subjects
Eighty kindergarten and first grade children attending
the P.K. Yonge Laboratory School of the University of Flor-
ida were selected. There were 40 females and 40 males.
One male child moved during the data collection period re-
sulting in a total sample of 79, 40 female and 39 male. All
subjects were Caucasian and participating in the regular
class program.
The population attending P.K. Yonge School is drawn
equally from five SES levels in a semi-urban population.
The students are selected for P.I. Yonge Laboratory School
to reflect both the ethnic and SES composition of the com-
munity. The five SES levels are as follows: Level I in-
come--<$6,000, Level II--$6,000 $10,499, Level III--
$10,500 $14,499, Level IV--$15,000 $20,999, Level V--
$21,000+.
All subjects were of at least normal intelligence as
measured by the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. The IQ
range was from 91 162, with the mean IQ 113.
Procedure
Thursday or Friday of the week prior to the data collec-
tion the experimenter visited the classroom for the day This
enabled the experimenters to become more familiar with the
classes' daily routine and physical layout. The experiment-
ers consisted of two senior undergraduate psychology majors
in addition to the primary investigator. The primary in-
vestigator collected data from two classrooms, the other
experimenters each collected data from one classroom.
Each of the four classrooms had one experimenter for
the period of one week. Each subject accompanied the ex-
perimenter to a small workroom adjacent to the classroom
for three separate testing sessions. Each session ranged
from 15-30 minutes. Each of the three sessions comprised
one aspect of the evaluation. The experimental condition
was broken down into three sessions to accommodate the
child's attention span. Session A consisted of the Pea-
body Picture Vocabulary Test; Session B consisted of the
ideational fluency tasks and the Imaginative Play Predis-
position Interview; Session C consisted of the Holtzman
Inkblot Test. The experimental tasks were presented to the
subject in a randomized sequence to account for order ef-
fects. All of the subjects in one classroom were processed
in one week, except for circumstances of school absence.
No two sets of evaluation instruments were administered in
one day.
Concurrent with the experimenter's data collection
from the students was data collection from the teachers.
Initially the primary investigator met with the teachers
to discuss the Playfulness Rating Scales, to explain the
instructions and to answer any questions. A copy of the
rating scale was left with the teachers. They were asked
to keep the rating scale in mind as they observed their
students for 2 weeks. A second session with the experi-
menter was held to further discuss the rating scale and to
answer questions. The teachers were then asked to rate the
children.
Means of Evaluation
Evaluation instruments for this study covered three
defined areas of inquiry: ideational fluency, playfulness
and imaginative play predisposition. In addition, a stan-
dardized picture vocabulary test was used to estimate in-
telligence.
Ideational fluency was examined utilizing two tasks:
(a) Product Improvement Task, and (b) The Monroe Language
Classification Test. A description of the tests is as fol-
lows. The Product Improvement Task is a two part test.
For Part I the subject was presented with a stuffed dog made
of brown plush material, approximately 7 inches long. The
dog had button eyes, a button black nose, a red tongue and
a red ribbon around its neck. The following instructions
were given:
Well, what have we here. It is a toy dog.
Now you tell me the most interesting, clev-
erest ways by which you can change this dog
(or: make him different) so that you and
other children would have more fun playing
with it. You just tell me how and I'll
write down your ideas.
The subject's responses were recorded for 1 minute 30 sec-
onds. For Part II the subject was presented with a cloth
doll, McCalls Pattern #5724. The doll was approximately
11 inches long with brown yarn hair and a blue denim shirt
and pants. The following instructions were given:
What else do we have here? It's a doll. Now
you go ahead and tell me the most interesting
and cleverest ways by which you can change
this doll so that it will be more fun to play
with.
The subject's responses were recorded for 1 minute 30 sec-
onds. See Appendix A for the tasks.
The Monroe Language Classification Test was a three
part test. The subject was given the following instruc-
tions: "Now this is the last thing we're going to do."
"Tell me all the animals you can think of as quickly as you
can." "Tell me all the things to eat you can think of."
"Tell me all the toys you can think of." Responses for
each category were recorded in a 1 minute 30 second time
block. See Appendix A for the tasks.
The Product Improvement task and the Monroe Language
Classification Test were developed and/or adapted by Lieber-
man (1964) in her work on divergent thinking in kindergarten
children. Ideational fluency, an aspect of divergent think-
ing, was selected to be examined in this study as it yielded
the results of greatest significance in her work. Lieber-
man (1964) utilized the following criteria for development
of her instruments: (a) applicability of existing tests,
or of specially constructed tests modeled after existing
tests, at the kindergarten level; (b) evidence for validity
and reliability of these tests from a pilot study for her
initial research project and from the existing literature.
Based on the work by Guilford (1951) and Torrance
(1960) on creative thinking and Gewirtz (1948) and Meyers
(1962) on verbal fluency in young children, Lieberman
(1964) developed the following tasks: Product Improvement,
Guilford's Plot Titles, and the Monroe Language Classifica-
tion Test. From the Monroe Test the objects selected were
Animals, Things to Eat, and Toys. The child was asked to
list as many as he could think of. The tasks are included
in Appendix A.
Validation and Reliability of the Tests
Lieberman's (1964) final selection of the tests was
made after a pilot study with 14 subjects attending a pri-
vate kindergarten in New York City. The age range was from
4-6 to 6-3 years, the average being 5-0 years. Meaningful
scores were obtained for ideational fluency from all three
tests. She noted that teacher conferences provided a pre-
liminary validity check since the children who scored high
on the divergent thinking factors were judged creative by
the teachers.
Lieberman developed a special scoring guide for the
ideational fluency tasks (1964). (See Appendix B.) As far
as possible, the rationale was modeled after existing tests
by Torrance and Guilford. The introduction of the new prod-
uct, namely the doll and the original text of the stories,
called for specific guidelines for the answers obtained.
Interscorer reliability for the divergent thinking scores
was established in the Lieberman study on a sample of 20
records. The Pearson r's were .99 for ideational fluency
on all three tasks.
Data on Validity from the Literature
The evidence about the tasks measuring ideational flu-
ency is as follows. (a) For Product Improvement Torrance
(1960) claimed face validity and gave scoring categories on
the basis of a sample of 146 elementary school children,
grades 1-6. (b) Analysis of the Monroe Language Classifi-
cation Test showed a factor loading of ideational fluency
(Meyers et al., 1962) and comparable tests of object naming
also showed loadings on ideational fluency (Bereiter, 1961;
Guilford & Christensen, 1956).
Data on Reliability from the Literature
For the Product Improvement Task Torrance (1960) men-
tioned the interchangeability of a toy monkey with the toy
dog and cited interscorer reliability in the .80's, hence
the selection of a dog in Lieberman (1964). Meyers et al.
(1962) reported a reliability of .62 based on intercorrela-
tions of the three parts for the Monroe Language Classifi-
cation Test (Animals, Toys, Things to Eat).
The Playfulness Scale
Playfulness was evaluated utilizing the Playfulness
Scale developed by Lieberman (1964). (See Appendix C.)
This scale consists of five subscales corresponding to the
five behavior traits of physical spontaneity, manifest joy,
sense of humor, social spontaneity, and cognitive spontane-
ity. Each scale is divided into an A and B part, referring
to the quantity and quality of the trait measured. These
subdivisions were made in order not to contaminate frequency
with degree or intensity of the trait measured. It was
found that the further refinement was considered helpful to
the raters (Lieberman, 1962). The division also indicated
no prior assumption that quantity and quality were related.
Ratings are made on a five point scale. Descriptive labels
for the points on the scale are given, as are sample behav-
ior items for each trait to be rated. The format of the
scale was modeled after Beller's (1955) instrument for as-
sessing dependence, independence and aggression in young
children through teacher ratings (Lieberman, 1964). As a
check on validity, Lieberman (1964) included two questions
not related to the behavior indices for playfulness. These
scales asked for an evaluation of the child's intelligence
and his physical attractiveness.
Reliability coefficients, obtained from the ratings of
the two teachers for the five traits and considered quali-
tatively and quantitatively, ranged with a Spearman-Brown
correction from .66 to .86 with a mean of .70 (Lieberman,
1964). Intercorrelations between the playfulness traits
ranged from .61 between sense of humor and physical spon-
taneity to .86 between manifest joy and cognitive spontane-
ity (Lieberman, 1964). When centroid factors were ex-
tracted, four of the five playfulness traits (cognitive
spontaneity, social spontaneity, manifest joy, sense of hu-
mor) had loadings in the middle .80's on the first factor.
Physical spontaneity had a loading of .78. Factors 2 and 3
accounted for little of the variance with factor loadings
ranging from .092 to .379. Lieberman (1964) concluded from
this that Playfulness could be conceptualized as a unitary
trait.
The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test
This test is an individual intelligence test for ages
2 1/2 to 18 years. It is designed to provide a well stan-
dardized estimate of a subject's verbal intelligence (Dunn,
1965). This test was chosen for its ease of administration
and previously noted appropriateness (Lieberman, 1964). As
an instrument using verbal stimuli and nonverbal responses,
it aims at an assessment of verbal comprehension and learned
information. It is, therefore, considered sufficiently dif-
ferent to provide some control measure for the scores on the
ideational fluency tasks (Lieberman, 1964). Also testing
time is short, which is an important consideration for use
with kindergarteners.
The test consists of 150 plates. Each plate has four
well drawn and unambiguous pictures from which the subject
is asked to choose one response to the examiner's question.
The subject need only point. The score is the number of
correct answers. The manual suggests starting points for
different age levels.
With respect to the difficulties experienced with mea-
sures of intelligence below age 6, the evidence cited for
validity and reliability can be considered satisfactory. A
correlation of .79 with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
Children has been obtained for a group of above average
elementary school children. Reliability coefficients are
given by age level and are .73 and .67 for ages 5 and 6,
respectively (Dunn, 1965).
Measuring Imaginative Play Predisposition
Singer (1973) notes that a major effort aimed at ar-
ticulating more precisely what goes into imaginative tenden-
cies in children and adults has grown out of the use of pro-
jective techniques. Projective techniques utilize the
the presentation of relatively ambiguous stimuli to indi-
viduals who are then required to tell stories about pic-
tures or to give associations to abstract inkblots, etc.
These ambiguous testing instruments were developed in an
effort to provide behavioral assessment of underlying in-
clinations. Singer (1968) views projective techniques as a
method to tap ongoing behavioral tendencies in the individ-
ual. The use of projective methods for studying imagination
has generally involved what the content reveals about spe-
cific conflict areas, as can be seen from the widespread
use of the Thematic Apperception Test or various forms of
association tests (Singer, 1973). A different approach has
developed using the Rorschach Inkblot Method. Rorschach
(1942) provided a major insight by noting that the tendency
to produce movement or color responses when looking at the
inkblots tapped not so much the specific content of con-
flicts or needs, but rather measured a broad trend toward
reliance on imagination or an open expression of emotional-
ity as a major tendency (Singer, 1973). Rorschach proposed
that all human experience could be measured along an intro-
version-extratension dimension and that the ratio of the
human movement (M) responses in inkblots to the color re-
sponses in producing associations was a fundamental way of
tapping this predisposition (Rorschach, 1942). On the whole,
there seems to be considerable support in research for at
least some aspects of Rorschach's interpretation of the
human movement responses as a measure of imaginativeness in
both children and adults (Singer, 1960, 1968, 1973). Singer
(1973) links the development of the human movement (M) re-
sponse to the internalization of speech in the form of
heightened imagery and fantasy. This provides the basis for
the choice of the inkblot method as one approach to estimat-
ing imaginative predisposition to play. Normative data with
the original set of Rorschach inkblots indicate that chil-
dren show relatively few such movement responses before the
age of 6 or 7 (Ames et al., 1974). It was necessary, then,
to find another technique appropriate to a younger popula-
tion. The Holtzman Inkblot Test (Holtzman, 1961), however,
provides useful norms on performance of children as young
as 5 years. The data from the Holtzman Inkblots strongly
support the importance of the movement response as an index
of ideational tendencies (Singer, 1973).
The Holtzman Inkblots were developed in an attempt to
provide a projective technique with psychometrically sound
scoring procedures for responses to inkblots while also pre-
serving the rich qualitative projective material of the
Rorschach (Holtzman et al., 1961). The Holtzman Inkblots
consist of 47 cards (45 are utilized in the score, 2 are
practice cards). The subject is required to provide one
response to each card. The cards are then scored, utilizing
a standard format. Various studies have examined the com-
parability of the Holtzman Inkblot responses and the
Rorschach Inkblot responses. Most of the recognized
Rorschach responses can be reproduced by configural scoring
utilizing the basic elements coded in the Holtzman technique
(Holtzman et al., 1961). Haggard, cited in Holtzman (Holtz-
man et al., 1961), utilizing multivariate analysis, found
significant correlations across eight scores in the responses
of high school students. Important to note is that the move-
ment response was one of the eight scores examined. Holtz-
man et al. (1961) conclude that these results indicate quite
conclusively that the Rorshach and Holtzman systems have a
great deal in common as far as the underlying meaning of
their respective variables is concerned. Holtzman et al.
(1961) provide an equivalency table for converting movement
(M) responses according to the Klopfer method of Rorschach
scores as Holtzman Inkblot scores of M>l plus H>1.
Biblow (1973) utilized the Holtzman Inkblots to deter-
mine imaginative predisposition with fifth grade children.
Utilizing the blots, fantasy level was determined upon the
basis of the score for movement responses to 20 selected
cards. Gottlieb (1973) also utilized the movement (M) score
on one-half of the Holtzman Inkblots, Form A, with elemen-
tary school and junior high school age children, as an as-
pect of imaginative predisposition evaluation.
The movement (M) response on the Holtzman blots is
scored for inanimate, as well as animate, movement.
Rorschach (1942), however, stated that movement (M)
responses applied only to human or animal (animate) move-
ment. In an effort to examine the introversive elements
articulated by Rorschach as related to imagination in the
individual, this study examines different combinations of
scores of the Holtzman blots: M scores alone and M>1 plus
H>l.
The odd-numbered cards, Form A, of the Holtzman Ink-
blots were used in this study. Odd-even reliability coef-
ficients for the procedure were sufficient to utilize one-
half of the instrument to allow for the kindergarteners'
limited attention span. The correlation coefficients for
each of the responses for the 5 year old population are as
follows: Movement (M) .80, Human (H) .79.
Singer has developed an interview for assessing imag-
inative play predisposition (Singer, 1973). (See Appendix
D.) He has noted that it is possible to get adequate data
from children as young as kindergarten age. The questions
are as follows: (a) What is your favorite game? What do
you like to play the most? (b) What game do you like to
play best when you are all alone? What do you like to do
best when you are all alone? (c) Do you ever have pictures
in your head? Do you ever see make-believe things with pic-
tures in your mind or think about them? What sort of
things? (d) Do you have a make-believe friend? Do you
have an animal or toy or make-believe person you talk to
or take along with you? Did you ever have one, even though
you don't any more?
The key in scoring these items has to do with the de-
gree to which the child's report indicates the introduction
of symbolic play or make-believe. The questionnaire was
scored on a five point scale (range 0 4 positive imagin-
ative responses). Singer (1973) notes that most children
do not receive a score higher than 2 or 3. He concludes
that scores of 0 1 indicate low imaginative play predis-
position, while scores of 2 or more indicate high imagina-
tive play predisposition.
CHAPTER IV
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
Inter-rater Reliability
The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test was scored accord-
ing to manual instructions, and the subjects received an IQ
score.
The Monroe Language Classification Test was scored
blind by three raters. Two raters were senior undergrad-
uate psychology majors; one rater was a first year graduate
student in clinical psychology. The raters participated in
a training session with the primary investigator. The rat-
ers then independently rated all the protocols. Inter-rater
reliability was established utilizing a Spearman Correla-
tion Coefficient. For the Monroe Language Classification
Test inter-rater reliability for the three raters was as
follows: r = .99, r = .97, r = .97.
The Imaginative Play Predisposition Interview responses
were scored blind following the same procedures as described
for the Monroe Language Classification Test. Inter-rater
reliability was established utilizing the Spearman Correla-
tion Coefficient. Inter-rater reliability for the Imagina-
tive Play Predisposition Interview was as follows: r = .80,
r = .83, r = .82.
The Product Improvement Task was rated by two senior
undergraduate psychology majors. The procedure for rating
was described above for the Monroe Language Classification
Test. Inter-rater reliability was established utilizing
the Spearman Correlation Coefficient. The inter-rater re-
liability was r = .96.
The Holtzman Inkblot Tests were scored by three ad-
vanced graduate students in clinical psychology who had ex-
perience in administering and scoring projective tests and
one less advanced graduate student in clinical psychology.
Each rater participated in a training session with the pri-
mary investigator to learn how to score the Holtzman blots
for human and movement responses. Each rater then scored
approximately 20 protocols (one rater scored 19). The pri-
mary investigator served as a standard with which the
raters' scores were compared for inter-rater reliability.
Four of the protocols were randomly selected for comparison
as follows. Each rater scored five protocols and one was
randomly selected for comparison prior to completing the
remaining protocols. The standard then independently rated
the target protocol. Differences in scoring were discussed
but the scoring was unchanged. This procedure was followed
until all the protocols were completed. A Tau B was uti-
lized for calculating inter-rater reliability. Reliabil-
ities were as follows:
Rater 1 Human Responses T = .94
Movement Responses T = .86
Rater 2 Human Responses T = .83
Movement Responses T = .99
Rater 3 Human Responses T = .72
Movement Responses T = .90
Rater 4 Human Responses T = .88
Movement Responses T = .93
It was hypothesized that there would be significant re-
lationships between imaginative play predisposition and
playfulness, with the relationship between the subitem cog-
nitive spontaneity and imaginative play predisposition as
most notable. In fact, the only relationships of signifi-
cance generated by the regression equations were between
cognitive spontaneity and imaginative play predisposition.
Imaginative play predisposition measured by the Singer in-
terview plus Holtzman movement score was significant at the
.005 level with the quantitative index for cognitive spon-
taneity. The slope of the regression was negative. Imag-
inative play predisposition as measured by the Singer inter-
view plus Holtzman movement response was significant at the
.05 level with the qualitative index for cognitive spontane-
ity. The slope of the regression was positive. Imaginative
play predisposition as measured by the Singer interview plus
the Holtzman movement plus human response was significantly
related to the quantitative index for playfulness. The
slope of the regression was negative.
There was no evidence for a significant relationship
between ideational fluency and playfulness.
Hypotheses I and II
Measuring Imaginative Play Predisposition
With respect to the interview task designed by Singer,
he suggests that frequency of high (2 or greater) and low
(0 1) imaginative play predisposition would be evenly
divided in a normal population. In this study the sample
was divided approximately in half (38 high, 49 low) with
the high imaginative play predisposition defined as a score
of 2 or greater and the low imaginative play predisposition
defined as a score of 0 or 1. (See Table 1 for distribu-
tion of scores.) Imaginative play predisposition was re-
lated to sex in this sample. Females scored higher than
males (F<.01).
Another question raised by this study was the appropri-
ate scoring technique for the Holtzman Inkblot Test. Spear-
man Correlation Coefficient for the relationship between the
movement responses alone and the combination human plus
movement responses was r = .78, suggesting that at this age
the responses are essentially the same. The relationship be-
tween the imaginative play predisposition and the Holtzman
movement responses was r = .14 and the imaginative play pre-
disposition and Holtzman human plus movement responses
I
R = .22; neither relationship was significant. This lack
of correlation suggests that the Imaginative Play Predis-
position Interview and the Holtzman Inkblot Test may still
be aspects of the complex phenomenon of imaginative play
predisposition but are clearly not measuring the same com-
ponents. For the purposes of this study both types of scor-
ing were examined in a linear regression model to examine
relationships between the Holtzman Inkblot score and the
other variables. Both movement and movement plus human re-
sponses were positively related to age: imaginative play
predisposition, movement alone: F<.O1; imaginative play
predisposition, movement plus human response: F<.005. This
would suggest that the older the child the higher the score
on the Holtzman task. This would confirm developmental
trends noted by Holtzman (cited in Hill, 1972). Others have
suggested (Ames et al., 1974) that this developmental trend
is evident in Rorschach movement (M) responses as well. This
introspective aspect of development does perhaps develop
over time. When the interview score and the Holtzman Ink-
blot score were combined it also reflected the relationship
to age: imaginative play predisposition, Holtzman movement
along/age: F<.005; imaginative play predisposition, Holtz-
man movement plus human response/age: F<.001. This may
suggest that the age effect in the Holtzman score is so
strong that the combined imaginative play predisposition is
age related. Imaginative Play Predisposition scores without
I
the Holtzman component were not age related. Singer (1966),
however, suggests that this trait is not age related but
rather is a quantifiable entity within an individual, which
may show development over time but should not directly be
affected by age. This raises some interesting questions.
If indeed, as Singer suggests, there is an imaginative play
predisposition and it can be measured by his interview task
without age demonstrating a clear relationship then the
utility of including the Holtzman measure with its strong
relationship to age is questionable. It would seem that
adding this measure, although hypothesized theoretically as
a component of imaginative play predisposition, is so af-
fected by developmental trends as to perhaps eliminate or
confound the true imaginative play predisposition. On the
other hand one could question whether or not this is an
artifact of the age range examined. The frequency of move-
ment (M) responses shows more marked change in these years
than at other ages. In Holtzman's normative data cited in
Hill (1972), 5 year olds at the 48th percentile generated
a raw score of 9; first graders at the 50th percentile gen-
erated a raw score of 14. Norms on fourth graders reflected
a raw score of 24 at the 50th percentile which was equiva-
lent to percentile norms for the average adult population.
This could indicate that the Holtzman Inkblot Test for a
younger population is too heavily affected by developmental
trends to serve as an adequate measure of imaginative play
predisposition for these early years. The Holtzman task,
however, was selected as it seemed more appropriate than a
Rorschach at these early years. A more suitable method of
examining imaginative play predisposition needs to be de-
veloped for these early years.
Hypothesis III
Measuring Playfulness
Lieberman's (1964) scale measures five separate aspects
of playfulness on both a quality and quantity dimension.
Each subdivision--physical spontaneity, manifest joy, social
spontaneity, cognitive spontaneity--can receive a rating
from 1 to 5 for each (quality, quantity). A child's com-
bined quality of playfulness score, therefore, has the range
of 5 25. A child's combined quantity of playfulness score
has the range of 10 50 (quality + quantity). In this
sample the playfulness scores did not distribute themselves
adequately over the range to reflect both high and low play-
fulness scores. Over 50% (n=58) received scores of 36 or
greater. As can be seen in Table 3 the sample reflected al-
most no children with low playfulness scores. When the
scores were broken down by SES levels (see Table 4) it can
be seen that this trend was evident in all SES levels.
The finding of high playfulness scores raises some in-
teresting questions. Utilizing Lieberman's instrument it
is evident that Lieberman's (1964) results were replicated.
As Lieberman's (1964) sample also reflected a high playful-
ness bias. The teachers were able, in rating the children,
to utilize all five points on each of the subdivisions (some
students did receive scores less than 3); however, the ma-
jority of students received scores of 3 or greater on all
the subdivisions. Two of the teachers were able to utilize
all five points on the Lieberman scale. The remaining two
teachers utilized four out of the five points on the Lieber-
man scale. Tables 5-8 illustrate the four teachers' ratings.
The playfulness rating scale requires further examination
on more samples of children before playfulness as a con-
struct can be further explored.
Hypothesis IV
Measuring Ideational Fluency
Scores were obtained in a distribution which ranged
from low to high for ideational fluency. The point range,
however, was so limited as to raise questions as to the
utility of the methodology for further research. With a
range of scores found in this population from 1 to 38, over
50% of the population (n=44) scored in the middle range from
the scores of 16 25. With a restricted scoring range of
less than 40 points, a majority of scores within the range
of 9 points makes it difficult to utilize the instruments
as designed by Lieberman (1964). (See Table 9.) In order
to measure the construct of ideational fluency more effec-
tively tasks need to be designed which better discriminate
between the high and low dimensions of ideational fluency.
Hypothesis V, VI and VII
It was proposed that given the possibility of measur-
ing the three constructs adequately ideationall fluency,
imaginative play predisposition, playfulness) comparisons
between scores on each of the three would yield significant
relationships. It was suggested that high scores on playful-
ness would be related to high scores on imaginative play
predisposition and ideational fluency. This was suggested
given the theoretical underpinnings of the constructs. Due
to the limited range of scores on the ideational fluency
instruments and the difficulty in establishing any subjects
in the low range of playfulness, regression functions gen-
erated few significant relationships between imaginative
play predisposition, playfulness and ideational fluency.
In order to further examine the lack of significant re-
lationships between imaginative play and playfulness, the
top twenty imaginative play scores and the lowest twenty
imaginative play scores were examined in relationship to
their corresponding playfulness scores (See Table 10 and
11). It was hoped that by examining this subsample of the
population some further information might be gained regard-
ing the relationship between imaginative play and
playfulness. The score utilized to determine imaginative
play was the combined interview and Holtzman movement plus
human response. A low imaginative play response was de-
fined as <4; a high imaginative play score was defined as
>17. Graphs were plotted comparing imaginative play to a
total playfulness score, a combined playfulness quantity
score, a combined playfulness quality score, as well as
each of the ten components of the combined scores. There
were no apparent differences in the distributions. In sum-
mary, the only significant relationships generated by the
regression equations were between imaginative play predis-
position and cognitive spontaneity.
Discussion
Extensive theoretical work and research in play behav-
ior in children has generated a vast and less than cohesive
body of literature. Although play behavior has aroused the
interest of many, there is neither a cohesive theory nor an
adequate methodology to tackle this complex behavior in
human children. One difficulty has been the fact that our
theorizing has extended beyond adequate empirical data to
support it. Another difficulty has been the diversity of
approaches to the problem. Investigators have tended to
follow their own isolated routes of inquiry without compar-
ing their work. Rather than being able to compare and con-
trast constructs, new ones are formed. This study attempted
to provide empirical data to support two current constructs
and theoretical perspectives within play behavior. Second,
it attempted to compare two different approaches to play
behavior in an effort to generate some unity in theoretical
conceptualization.
The first task, that of providing empirical support
to two current theoretical constructs in the current lit-
erature, met with variable outcome. Jerome Singer's (1973)
work on imaginative play predisposition is a relatively
recent contribution to theoretical conceptualization of
the role of imaginative play behavior in the development
of the individual. This study, by demonstrating that
imaginative play predisposition can be measured utilizing
Singer's interview technique in a SES heterogeneous pop-
ulation of kindergarten and first graders, yields greater
validity to the construct as well as reliability for the
instrument with this population. The questions raised
regarding the utility of the Holtzman Inkblot Test with
this population are equally important. Given the high
relationship between age/developmental factors and the
resultant Holtzman Inkblot scores, one is inclined to
doubt the utility of the instrument for this population
in studying imaginative play predisposition. Furthermore,
if imaginative play predisposition is a quasi-personality
trait or an aspect of personality development as Singer
postulates, then one is inclined to question the Holtzman
Inkblot Test as an instrument for measurement as it is so
effected by age.
A third question that should be raised is regarding the
developmental course of the imaginative play predisposi-
tion. Is the development so rapid during these years that
it is difficult to measure or is it truly a trait in the
individual or only an artifact of age or development. A
further question to be asked regarding methodology is that
if the Holtzman Inkblot Test and the Imaginative Play Pre-
disposition Interview are so poorly correlated can one
task be substituted for the other in inquiries with older
age children where the Imaginative Play Predisposition In-
terview is inappropriate. The above mentioned questions
underscore several needs in this area. First of all,
further work needs to explore the definition of the imag-
inative play predisposition in order to generate a more
appropriate methodology to examine it. Second, work in
defining imaginative play predisposition and its methodol-
ogy must be examined over more populations to provide a
firmer data base for the construct.
Although Lieberman's (1964) results regarding playful-
ness were essentially replicated, i.e., teachers rated children
as discriminable and in the upper range of playfulness, it
is not possible to conclude anything from those data. With-
out establishing validity, other than face validity, and re-
liability of the construct, it is difficult to conclude what
the playfulness scale actually measures. Playfulness as a
construct is theoretically interesting and potentially use-
ful. Intuitively it addresses many of the concerns raised
regarding play behavior by incorporating cognitive, social
and affective components. Before further empirical explora-
tion is possible, however, considerable basic methodolog-
ical research must take place in order to determine the
validity and reliability of the construct. One research pos-
sibility would be to utilize trained observational data on
children's play behavior with teachers' and/or other trained
observers ratings with the playfulness rating scales. Es-
tablishment of adequate validity and reliability of the
Playfulness scale would permit it to serve as an efficient
research tool.
The second problem attempted in this study, to compare
two aspects of play behavior in order to provide some unity
of theoretical conceptualization, yielded significant rela-
tionships which are intriguing. In turn these results raise
further questions. Given the limited utility of the Lieber-
man (1964) playfulness instrument caution must be used in
interpreting the results. In light of this caution, how-
ever, the relationships of significance were found between
imaginative play predisposition and the cognitive
spontaneity subset of playfulness. As the cognitive spon-
taneity dimension of playfulness most directly addresses
imaginative play, this may in fact be the only aspect of
playfulness which is related to Singer's (1973) imaginative
play predisposition. Of interest, however, was the fact
that the slope of the regressions for the relationship be-
tween frequency of cognitive spontaneity and imaginative
play were negative, whereas the slope of the regression
representing the relationship between the quality of cog-
nitive spontaneity and imaginative play was positive. One
might intuitively assume that the slopes would both be pos-
itive. The question on the Lieberman (1966) instrument for
measuring the quality of congitive spontaneity directly ad-
dresses the degree of imagination demonstrated by the child
in expressive and dramatic play. These significant relation-
ships indicate an important direction for further empirical
investigation.
An enormously frustrating task for the individual in-
terested in examining play behavior is the lack of an ade-
quate methodology for research. Although fascinating, play
behavior is complex. Theoretical conceptualization surround-
ing play is diverse and equally complex.
The limited significant results in this study raise
three basic questions. The first question is whether or not
measures of the constructs have any empirical relationship
to each other. The theoretical bases of the constructs
point to a relationship which was only minimally supported
by the data. The second question raised by the results of
this study is whether or not the constructs as defined are
adequate for further empirical work. The aforementioned
question is a methodological concern. The evidence for re-
liability and validity of the instruments currently assumed
to measure the constructs may be inadequate to make any
clear statements regarding the existence of the phenomena.
The third question raised regards the logical processes of
research: the inductive process vs. the deductive process.
There are two opposing schools of thought concerning
the most effective methods for integrating diverse data.
The deductive school states that theories should be formu-
lated first and then tested by experiment. The inductive
method is to experiment first and to let the theories emerge
from the data. The difference between the two schools is
not so much the presence or absence of theory in research,
but the way in which theory is developed.
The deductive method provides a certain elegance in re-
search by which comprehensive theories generate the defini-
tion of constructs. These constructs are then operationally
defined and reliable and valid instruments are developed to
measure observable behavior. Dataare gathered in an effort
to empirically support the theoretical postulates. The dif-
ficulty in this logical process is exemplified by the area
of play. Theoretical development has far surpassed
methodological development. In psychology where phenomena
are diverse and complex this presents a serious dilemma.
The current trend in psychological research is to narrow
one's focus to examine a small amount of relatively simple
data.
A second approach in current psychological research
is to utilize the inductive method. Sidman states: in-
tensive cultivation of an area of research by an alert ob-
server will inevitably bring out interrelations among the
phenomena comprising the area (Sidman, 1960, p. 15). In
this method a large number of experiments must be conducted
with the hypothesis that order will emerge. This requires
a basic faith in the orderliness of behavior.
The present research most closely followed the deduc-
tive method. Based on theoretical conceptualizations, re-
lationships between the constructs of imaginative play pre-
disposition, playfulness and ideational fluency were hypoth-
esized. Conclusions from the data are limited by the ab-
sence of careful methodological development. This can be
seen in the lack of clear operational definitions of the
constructs, and the absence of reliable and valid instru-
ments to measure observable behavior.
The course for future research can be guided by les-
sons from both the inductive and deductive methods of re-
search. Inductively more observational data need be col-
lected to generate the definition of play behavior and its
parameters. Deductively, the definitions of play behavior
need to be tested empirically in relationship to the exten-
sive theoretical literature existing regarding play. Re-
search should establish the reliability and validity of the
instruments presumed to measure behavior related to the con-
structs of imaginative play and playfulness. This could be
accomplished utilizing careful observational data on chil-
dren's play behavior.
Investigation of children's play behavior offers the
researcher the potential for greater understanding of de-
velopment: affective, cognitive, social and psychopatho-
logical. It is not surprising that play has captured the
interest of philosophers, sociologists, anthropologists, as
well as psychologists. As with so many areas in psychology,
theoretical work reaches far beyond the data base. Each
empirical investigation provides a few small steps to close
that gap. Often of greater importance are the new questions
which are raised. This study although providing few an-
swers, clearly points to some important directions for fu-
ture research.
67
TABLE 1
DISTRIBUTION OF IMAGINATIVE PLAY PREDISPOSITION
INVENTORY (SINGER) FREQUENCY OVER SES LEVELS
Imaginative SES Level
SES Level
Play
Predisposition
I II III IV V
Interview Score
**
0 1 (.07) 3 (.19) 2 (.13) 3 (.19) 3 (.17)
1 4 (.29) 7 (.44) 6 (.40) 5 (.31) 7 (.39)
2 5 (.36) 4 (.25) 4 (.27) 5 (.31) 4 (.22)
3 3 (.21) 1 (.06) 3 (.20) 1 (.06) 3 (.17)
4 1 (.07) 1 (.06) 0 2 (.13) 1 (.06)
Number of subjects.
Number of subjects.
**Percentage of N for each SES level.
TABLE 2
DISTRIBUTION OF COMBINED IMAGINATIVE PLAY PREDISPOSITION
AND HOLTZMAN INKBLOT TEST SCORES OVER SES LEVELS
Combined SES Level
Score
I II III IV V
**
0 5 5 (.36) 3 (.19) 4 (.27) 4 (.25) 9 (.50)
6 10 4 (.29) 5 (.31) 5 (.33) 2 (.13) 3 (.17)
11 15 0 4 (.25) 2 (.13) 3 (.19) 3 (.17)
16 20 4 (.29) 2 (.13) 3 (.20) 4 (.25) 1 (.056)
>. 21 1 (.07) 2 (.13) 1 (.07) 2 (.13) 2 (.11)
Number of subjects.
**
Percentage of N for SES level.
69
TABLE 3
PLAYFULNESS SCORE FREQUENCIES
1 -5 0
6 10 0
11 15 1
16 20 2
21- 25 3
26 30 9
31- 35 6
36 40 25
41 45 26
46 50 7
TABLE 4
DISTRIBUTION OF PLAYFULNESS OVER SES LEVEL
SES Level
Playfulness SES Level
Score
I II III IV V
1-5 0 0 0 0 0
6 10 0 0 0 0 0
11 15 0 1 0 1 0
16 20 0 0 1 0 1
21 25 1 1 0 0 1
26 30 1 2 2 1 4
31 35 1 0 2 2 2
36 40 4 4 7 5 4
41 45 4 6 2 6 6
46 50 3 2 1 1 0
71
TABLE 5
TEACHER B PLAYFULNESS RATINGS
>4
>O >O +J > >1 >O
40 4J 4J 4J 4J 4J 4J
3 4 .-4 5 .4 H ) 4*4 a) -44
-4 a 4 0 ) > )
+j 4J 10 : 1 rA 4-)> *
5q B5 3f 3 5 4 3 3 d i 4 J
0 0 n3 >1 >1 u 0 u 0l ( d fO U O t3 o ro tr o r0
S5 o I 5 3 O 3 o
(n z i xi P4 cn 0 04 ffi Ol i 01u In oi
1 5 4 5 5 4 5 5 5 5 5
2 4 4 5 4 4 4 3 4 4 4
3 4 4 5 4 4 4 5 5 4 4
4 3 3 3 4 4 3 3 4 5 3
5 5 5 4 3 3 2 4 4
6 5 4 5 4 5 5 4 3 3 3
7 5 4 5 5 5 4 5 3 5 4
8 4 4 5 4 4 4 3 4 3 3
9 3 3 5 4 5 4 3 3 4 4
10 4 4 5 4 3 3 4 4 3 4
11 5 4 5 5 4 3 5 4 5 5
12 5 5 4 4 4 3 4 3 55
13 4 4 4 4 5 5 4 3 4 5
14 5 5 4 5 4 4 5 5 5 5
15 4 4 5 4 4 4 4 5 4 5
16 3 4 5 5 4 3 4 4 5 5
17 5 4 4 5 5 5 5 5
18 3 2 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 4
19 4 3 5 4 4 4 4 5 4 4
20 4 3 4 3 3 4 4 54 5
TABLE 6
TEACHER A PLAYFULNESS RATINGS
>1 4-3 O > >1 > >1 >0
4J 4 1 .-4 45 40 4- 45-
-, 4 4- 4 -,4 -r -H
u 4 u -4 4 U 44 3 4 -4 4. 4J -4
) Q) -.4-3 4J -4 4J -i Cl CX 4 4J 4 -A (0 4J -) m ( -H 4 p .--V 4 .I
0 o> 0 M > o>o ) C rd Uo 0 reol 0 rd
0 0 03 0 0 0
00 U) (I O( Pa 4n O( -)m (31 U) (n (' U) m cq u cn 02 u U) cy
21 4 5 4 4 5 5 5 4 5 5
22 4 4 4 4 5 5 5 4 5 5
23 2 3 3 3 3 2 3 2 4 4
24 4 2 4 4 4 4 3 3 4 4
25 3 2 3 3 3 2 3 2 4 4
26 4 2 5 5 3 2 4 3 5 5
27 4 4 5 5 4 4 4 4 5 5
28 4 3 3 3 3 2 3 3 3 3
29 4 3 5 5 3 3 4 4 4 4
30 3 3 4 4 3 3 5 5 4 4
31 3 2 3 3 2 2 2 2 3 3
32 4 4 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5
33 2 2 4 4 5 5 4 4 4 4
34 3 4 3 3 3 2 5 5 5 5
35 4 3 4 4 3 2 4 4 4 4
36 4 4 4 4 5 4 5 5 5 5
37 3 4 5 5 2 2 4 3 4 4
38 3 3 5 5 4 3 4 4 4 4
39 3 2 4 4 3 2 4 4 2 2
40 4 3 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5
73
TABLE 7
TEACHER C PLAYFULNESS RATINGS
>1 > 4-O >1 >0 >0 >O >0
VJ +J -.4 4J a a -P -
-H 4 .*r .r *H -, 01 -H
Hr-1 > ) >1 Q) >1 ) > > >
4i 4 4 j 4 M J
-A H4J- 4J -H-V -H C4 0 -I4-' 4 *H rd J -w 4-* 1 H 44-P -H
SS > o > 0 rd >1 0>3 rd 0 o 0 0 ( 0 p o0
05 0 4 : 0 l 0 3 3 3 0 3D4 3 0 2 0 4 3
SP4 U) (DI 0 U DI -D 04) (nl cx U) CN In M Cn9O Um Ol U CO
41 3 5 3 2 4 3 3 2 2 2
42 3 2 5 5 4 2 5 5 4 4
43 4 3 3 4 3 2 2 2 4 4
45 3 3 3 3 3 3 2 2 3 2
46 1 4 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
47 4 2 5 5 5 5 4 4 5 5
48 4 2 5 5 4 4 4 4 4 4
49 3 2 3 3 3 2 3 2 2 2
50 3 3 3 2 3 3 3 2 3 4
51 1 5 3 2 3 3 3 2 3 3
52 4 4 5 5 3 3 2 3 4 4
53 4 5 5 5 4 3 4 5 5 5
54 3 3 4 4 3 3 4 4 4 4
55 1 2 1 1 4 4 2 1 1 3
56 5 1 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5
57 5 1 5 4 5 5 4 4 5 5
58 1 2 2 2 2 2 2 1 3 2
59 1 1 3 1 3 2 2 1 4 4
60 5 5 5 5 3 3 4 4 5 5
74
TABLE 8
TEACHER D PLAYFULNESS RATINGS
> .O >1 4C > > O >1 > O 0
61 5 5 4 4 444 5 5 3 4
62 5 *- ) -4 3 4 )
r)> HU H > >1 0) >1 0) > 0), >0 0
4-rl ( 4 >1 4 >, *, 4 '
63 4 3 u 5 4 3 l 4
)64 5 5 54J 4 r-l 43 4- 4 4 () -1 4- 4 3
65 5 5 5C n 3 3 4 q
> 0 > i0 O >1 M ) 0) 0 T ro tai 0 m
66 5 4 5 4 5 5 5 5 45 : 0 Q45
co z 1 I (D I cq yi oi c CX Ol En M Ol U) Mn Ol U co cy u U) CN
61 5 5 4 4 4 4 5 5 3 4
62 5 4 5 5 4 3 4 3 4 5
63 4 3 5 5 4 3 5 5 4 4
64 5 5 5 5 5 5 4 3 4 3
65 5 5 5 5 4 4 3 3 4 4
66 5 4 5 4 5 5 5 5 4 5
67 5 3 4 4 5 4 3 3 4 4
68 5 4 5 5 4 3 4 4 4 4
69 4 2 5 3 3 3 3 3 4 3
70 4 4 5 5 4 4 3 3 4 3
71 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 3 5 5
72 4 3 5 5 4 3 3 2 5 4
73 5 5 4 3 3 2 5 5 5 5
74 4 4 5 5 3 3 2 2 4 4
75 5 4 3 4 2 1 4 4
76 5 4 5 5 3 3 4 4 3 3
77 5 4 5 5 5 5 4 3 3 4
78 5 5 3 4 3 2 5 5 3 2
79 3 3 4 5 3 3 2 2 4 4
80 4 2 3 4 4 3 2 4 4 4
TABLE 9
DISTRIBUTION OF IDEATIONAL FLUENCY SCORES OVER SES LEVELS
Ideational SES Level
Fluency
Score I II III IV V
TABLE 9 (continued)
Ideational SES Level
Fluency
Score I II III IV V
29 0 1 0 1 3
30 0 2 2 3 2
15 2*(.14)** 2 (.13) 1 (.07) 3 (.19) 3 (.17)
16 25 9 (.64) 7 (.44) 11 (.73) 9 (.60) 9 (.50)
_ 26 3 (.21) 6 (.38) 3 (.20) 5 (.33) 7 (.39)
*Ideational Fluency scores.
**Percentage scores.
0
u
CO
z
0
H
H
C-O
0
ao
H
04
1-
H
z
H
H
P4
E-4
U
Fl.
z
0
H
H
O
H
oI
H
m m m H o o 0 N -
n -f o n -i O -i i- o r^ r- ^
4uauDWAOW ueWZI-OH
snld MaTA-xa;UI
uOTqTsodSTPaId
ABTd DATqUTBEUII
4U@UIaAOW u2eIZ4TOH
snld MOTAla UI
UOTq TSOdSTpaad
APTd @ATqeuTBemI
MaTAa-JUI
UOTqTSOdSTpE.ld
A Td aATqPuT6PWjI
ApTd paTrrquloz
AkT -run o 0
AeTd pauTqcuo3o N N
APTd TO~1o V 0
A4TTn6 Aq-pTUP4
-uods aATqTU6OD
AqTTun en Taupe
-uods ieToos
AqTqupenb A4T au
-uods IeTOOS
,I In
re uIn
A4TUBn6 JOUInH
AqT4lenb ournH
A4Tlen6 Aop
A4Tquenb Aor In In
AqrT"en AqTaupq
-uods TeOTssAqd
AqTquen6 Aqzaueq
-uods ieOTsAqd
.aqumnN o aCqns (N m
0 0 H-l 0 %D
Cl C
rC Me 0
0 C( H1 H
m 0
Oc~rlI
m
m H
0 H
(N (N
H-I CO MN In ) O %0 Om
r- Cm r-i H- (N (N C( (N Cn C
o c0 -1 Lf (N4 rC O N rM
(N r- I C( H- N H- H- H- (- H-q
in 0 14 in mo 'D H-1 0 C O
(N (Nq N -4 (N -4 N (q N H-4
Ln 0 Lin 0 Ln CY 0) m Ln 0
m Cl m 'IT rM m m Cl m
IT r Ln In i In In (CN
Ln 1* In Cl n I' In U a In CN
ml m IT Cl I (N m l In Ln
In m In Ln C I t In m
,r in n ml Ln (N (N Cl r CN
Lnm c nnn Ln fn m c Ln r
I L U) InP l Cl nLn C
I in In In rn l In In Ln
In Cl In Cl r C In) In l
1 m I:T (N Ii r( (N C 'IV T N
In Cl In) ml IQ, l IQ
u,2wnH
UGUIG9AOW UPUIZ4TOH
Q snld MsTAJtSUI
Suoq-TsodsTpajld
AIed eATe~eTBuuI
0
0 UaurIAoW uet2uizOH
snld MATAJ-TIUI
S UOTSOdSTpBjd
c ATed ATWUT6eU15I
U)
uoTqTsOdsTp@Id
4 AP d ATqPUTfBrUII
-4
l
> Aed p:4UT UI0
4
SXA-4TTuen6
Aeld pUT-rqwlo
H
A-4
APe-d To40J.
-uodsg AT4TTuboD
l A4Tqiren n AqT7auep
U)
A -uods eAT TUE6o
0
t ATi[enQ A OTa ue
4 -uods TUrTOS
T>1 uen d TAT-0oS
a4 4TIn3 aomuinH
)T~un<5 -lourniH
AqTlpen6 Xof
^Tlnuenfl Aor
A-4TT-4nr ATauE.C
-uodS TPDTsAqd
4TxquPnN 3AqTaLeu
-uods POT sAqT
aquinN ooaCqns
.D m m
Hrl re) r 0 'a M H q
kD m
C') r
C'
Sm
w, m
Hl
,-I
ri
HJ
0 (N M CO <3
S0 H-4 n -H 0
H H-- N
,-1 1- CM
O' 0 HD M 0
(N (N (N (N
Cn H 'IV (N ,l ( N r-I CN
, (N H (N" rH- (N (q (N (N
S C) irn Ln (N (N N r- c
" CN N IT '
in (N in n' Z "
in IV N in 'T ^
" CN in -i iLn n i
S ,F m i (No in r in m L
Ln m m C) i n m(
in in n Ln H-4 in in i in
Ln min ) m Ln Lin Ln Ln
c( (N (N in H 'n in T
' IT n 'T H T in in in
r- co m n (r' m m n .o 0c
IzT fm i-n in W kD W
0
U
u
U)
en
z
10
H
H
U)
O
U)
to
H
>4
H
H
U)
<
I--4
Ut
U)
U)
L)3
z
0
H
E
H
U)
0
U)
--4
2
H
a
<
UI)
a
H
U)
E3
Fti
r-
h
W >
^-1
H
mn ko
lm 0 Om
(N r4 (N ml
a
0
0
0
.H
co
0(
PI
,-y
C-
H
>4
c,
r1
4t
ar
0
CO
r-l
1-1
''uauaAOW UeUzITOH
snld MaTAeJOUI
uoTTSodsTrpaOd
APTd OATPeuTbeuwI
quauIOAOW umeuz4OH
snTd MO TA.-auqI
UOT4TSOdSTpaOd
ApId eATqeuTfuWI
MOTA-Ta UI
UOT4TSOdSTpa-ld
APTd @ATPUT6)PUII
AITd pauTquwOD
A4T1uren6
AeTd p@urquTo3
A eTd Teo01L
A4T-[n6 A4TauE4
-uods OATqTUBOD
AcTqupn6 AiTaupq
-uodS eATqTUBOD
AqTTpen6 qTaupq
-uods TpToOS
ArTqupen6 AqTaup
-uods i-rTOOS
A4TI-nb iownH
AqTIuenO JounH
A-4T-ueno Aoo
A;TTpn6 ATaOUuq
-uods TLOTSAqd
A-T quenf A; TOauP
-uods I-[oTsAqd
xaqnmN qoaCqns
Cl
CO' 0) C
(Ni (N H-
Cl
n m M
sa~Cl
H N Hl
Cl l (
'.0
'.0
(N H H Cl
\O
'.0
r- r-
'.0
\D
rk r-1
O rN
CN (
(N (N
' oN o 0 T ( -N '), rl om CO co
(N H- r-H ( (N (N cN CN ( r-4 H-4
,: I C N N C r-4 r-4 K' -4 Ch 00C -
C(N (N CN (N N (N (N (N N r- r-H (
CO co H (CN rN Cl (N CO I CO kO O)
U) 41 rU) U m m ir U) Ln UzC Cl C'
Lin IT rl i LI LT ULI LI L n 't
Ln rNl m Cl LI Ln Ir Ln z T Cl LI
Ln m I: T LIn ^r I LI Ln U) LI
Lu m Ln C 'U' 1 rCl Ln LI I c(N (N
' 'I'l Ln 4 'j- "T I-T Ln LI Ln (N '
LI LI I Ln LI Ln Ln lr IQ, LI Ln
Ln Ln LI I'a Ln Ln 'IT 'IT Ln Ln
^ Cl '' LIn LIn l'I (N
LIn LI Ln LI LI L Cl n LI ff (N C Q
(N Q' Ln k.0 rN H Cl N N
r-H n LI i r-H r-I r-H -i HN C C
.D
CO N
H- H-
'.0
'.0
a, N
Hl H
ueuinH
q'UUIrAOW uUIZ4TOH
snTd MaTAqaeUI
uoTqTsodsTpazd
P Td OATU.UTb6uII
ueWumAOW uemzqIOH
snId MaTAa34UI
UOT4TSOdsTpapI
APTd DATPUT6&eUII
MaTAX@-aUI
uoT TsodsTpoaid
ABTad DAT-4UTEPUI
4qETPnO
4PId pauTquuoD
A^T^uen5
AeTd pauTquloD
ePTd TeqOL
AqTT12n5 AqTDup4
-uods aAT.TU603
A4T-4Xuen6 AqT3u2e-
A4TIpn6 AqTaueq
-uods TPTOOS
-uods TlTOOS
A-TTueno owmnH
AqTquen6 aouinH
AqTirenb Aor
A TTTen6 Aq oup3
-uods leoTsAqd
ATquden ATSra Ue
-uods TIOTsATId
0
0 M 4
nm
~~C
m
Ln~
M ( -4
m
0
Ln 0 n
in or-
"In 4 (N 00 0 orI r-4
H-i l -H (N H-q (N H- (N
In m
rH Hl
O') CN H- (N 0 CN
(N (N C4 (N (N
O C 00 0 IT o0 (N 0) (n
CN (N (N ^, C) n CT rn
(,) m m In T In C' 'a
m m c -4 in 11r Ln m Cn
(N (N r-i I m In 'T m
r nm IIZV m^ r Ln 'IT Irv
m C T om v (N (q In
m m m C In C') m In
(M (N In l C) Ln Ln
0n H I-A n I zr In In
SIn m N IMn m mn 1' T
C -) r- H In In Ln In In
0 o Ln 0 o ro o r-
L) Ln n qon In I I- r-N -
laquinN qoa~qns
APPENDIX A
IDEATIONAL FLUENCY TASKS: INTERVIEW SCHEDULE
Introduction
"I have some things here to do and I'd like you to help me
with them. I want to find out how boys and girls do these
things, and whether they have fun doing them. So, I'm
going to ask you and (mention playmates where indicated) to
do these with me."
Product Improvement (after E. Paul Torrance)
1. "Well, what have we here. It is a toy dog. Now
you tell me the most interesting, cleverest ways by which
you can change this dog (or: make him different) so that
you and other children would have more fun playing with it.
You just tell me how and I'll write down your ideas."
Record answers and comments on form.
Time: 1'30"
If subject obviously runs out of responses, note
time and go on to Part 2.
If subject can give no responses, skip Part 2 and
go on to next item.
Only where subject is obviously shy, say: "All
right, let's leave this for a while and maybe
you'll think of something later." Return at
end of battery.
2. "What else do we have here? It's a doll. Now
you go ahead and tell me the most interesting and cleverest
ways by which you can change this doll so that it will be
more fun to play with."
Proceed as above.
Time: 1'30"
Monroe Language Classification Test (after C.E. Meyers
et al.)
"Now this is the last thing we're going to do."
"Tell me all the animals you can think of as quickly as
you can."
"Tell me all the things to eat you can think of."
"Tell me all the toys you can think of."
Record each category.
Time: 30" for each (extension to 1' consid-
ered). Praise child for cooperation.
"How did you like the things we did?"
Record answers.
APPENDIX B
IDEATIONAL FLUENCY TASKS: GUIDE FOR SCORING
This guide is divided into two parts according to the
two tasks: (1) Product Improvement and (s) the Monroe
Language Classification Test.
While specific instructions for relevancy and scoring
of responses are given separately for each task on the basis
of the answers obtained in this study, the following overall
guidelines for the divergent thinking factors of ideational
fluency, spontaneous flexibility, and originality are ap-
plicable.
Ideational Fluency. This is obtained by counting all
of the separate relevant responses given by subjects regard-
less of quality.
Product Improvement
Relevancy of a Response
1. The response must indicate that the instructions
have been understood, i.e., something needs to be done to
or with the object, resulting in a change in object of sit-
uation around it. Examples:
Relevant Irrelevant
Make him into a fox You can play with him
Make him dance Share him
(If he) barked a little Squeeze his hand
like alive
Add another doll to make To keep it the way it is
him brothers
Key back here (without
follow-up)
2. The response must indicate that the child accepts
the premise of change for the purpose stated, i.e., "fun to
play with" and that it connects in some way with the given
object. Answers indicating unrelated associational stream
or pure nonsense--i.e., change to anything at all, are not
acceptable. Examples:
Relevant Irrelevant
Make ears stand up Paper to write on or change
Make tail wag into nothing
Put bow in his head Make into telephone
Change into rabbit (for dog) Turn into table
Change into doctor (for doll)
3. A reply may be relevant for toy dog but not for
toy doll and vice versa. Also, in Part 2 (doll) change to
stimulus object of 1 is not acceptable. Examples:
Relevant Irrelevant
Make him into a lion (dog) Make him into a lion (doll)
Make him into a dolly (dog) Make him into a dog (doll)
4. An answer is relevant, irrespective of underlying
affect, if outcome is judged constructive. Examples:
Relevant Irrelevant
Cut off head and use the out- I'll break his head, arms,
side (clothing) to make everything
a toy watch I'll cut off his ears
Poke holes in his eyes Take out his eyes
I'll twist his leg
5. When by explicit statement or by implication, the
dog or toy-man has been made alive ("adaptation" in Tor-
rance's categories for flexibility), the following answers
are relevant:
He might chase a cat
Make him go to work
To do things on a ship (in a number of instances sub-
jects called doll "a sailor")
6. Part of a reply may be accepted as relevant. Ex-
ample:
Play with it--wrestle or (put him on his head)
6. If a child by demonstration, acting out or story-
telling makes changes implicit, the reply is relevant.
Examples:
"Woof, woof" as subject handles dog
"Give him a house for children to have more fun play-
ing, and ask the postman to fix him"
Scoring of Relevant Responses
When scoring for ideational fluency, the unit of enu-
meration is the thought unit, this coinciding with the clas-
sification unit. Therefore,
score as one (1)
1. When unit is logically connected and stated as such
by subject. Examples: Take off shirt and change into dif-
ferent costume; take this ribbon off and put furry thing
around him.
2. When the second part of the unit is an explanation
of the first. Examples: Make him like Robin Hood--jump up
and down; barked a little, like alive.
3. When the second part is an alternative. Example:
Change his face with a mask or skin or an animal.
4. When after an overall change has been suggested,
specific changes are detailed. Example: Change into col-
lie--different fur, white and brown, back all brown, long
mouth, sharp claws.
5. When changes are distinct but make up one context
or classification unit, i.e., one idea. Example: To get a
funny black and white clothes, brown hands, white nose,
black eyes, and a funny green hat.
score as zero (0)
1. When only agents of change are given without spec-
ifying any changes. Examples: Make abracadabra; key back
there; a fairy could change it.
2. When change back is specified. Example: Change
into real pig, goose, and then change back to normal.
Monroe Language Classification Test
Relevancy of a Response
Animals--real, imaginary, living, extinct, and generic.
Examples:
Relevant Irrelevant
Cow Houses
Dinosaur People
Things to Eat--everything edible, including general cate-
gories and fluids as well as food specified for animals.
Examples:
Relevant
Lamb chops Everyt
Meat a st
Grill Animal
Peanuts
Milk
Toys--a. All specific toys. Examples:
Relevant
Irrelevant
hing you can buy in
ore
s
Irrelevant
Dolls Doll corner
Dump truck Baby toys
Games
b. Objects that are prefaced by "toy" or by the sub-
ject saying "one could play with" or that are implicitly
understood to be handled in play situation. Examples:
Relevant
Clock
Mustache
People (in the form of dolls)
Telephone
c. Materials for play activities. Examples:
Relevant
Clay
Wood to make toys
d. Responses with no bearing on play situation are
to be considered irrelevant. Examples:
Bees and bats
Closet Toy
Daphne (Name of playmate)
Scoring of Relevant Responses: Ideational Fluency
Animals. No credit for duplicate responses. Count
separately male and female of species, and generic and
specific responses. Examples: Lion, Father Lion; fish,
tunafish.
Things to Eat. a. Count separately when subject
specifies different kinds of the same food. Examples:
plain cheese, velveeta cheese. b. Count as single re-
sponse when one dish or meal is implied. Examples: Spag-
hetti and meatballs, hamburger and french fries, supper,
breakfast.
Toys. a. Collective terms are counted as single re-
sponses. Example: games, b. Mere elaboration without
differentiation is a single response. Examples: car,
automobile car. c. Count as separate responses when sub-
ject specifies differences in the same toy. Examples:
baby dolls, big dolls; trucks, gasoline trucks.
APPENDIX C
PLAYFULNESS SCALE
Rating Instructions
As a teacher you know that children differ in many
ways--some are shy, some are friendly, some grab what they
want, others ask, or wait, for it.
In this study, we are interested in finding out how
children differ in the way they go about their play activ-
ities--how spontaneous, how cheerful, how "full-of-the-dev-
il" they are, and we hope to have your cooperation in this
work.
Attached you will find, therefore, a rating measure
made up of five scales which refer directly to a child's
behavior during play. You will note that each of the five
scales or questions has two parts. Part A of the question
aims to get at the frequency or quantity of the trait;
Part B tries to assess the quality of the trait shown. For
example, "how often does the child show joy" would be the
quantity of the trait, and "with what freedom of expression"
would be the quality of the trait.
We hope you will find it possible and worthwhile to
look at the children in your group along the traits
suggested in the rating scales and give us your evaluation
of them.
We are also interested in finding out what your impres-
sion is of the child's intelligence and physical attractive-
ness, and we would like you to give us your estimate of
these as well.
When you rate the children, you will, of course, want
to compare them with one another as well as keep in mind a
general standard for these traits in kindergarteners.
It is easier and better to rate all children first on
one trait (or question), and then do the same for each of
the six other questions. The sheets for marking down your
ratings have, therefore, been set up for the different
traits.
There will be twelve ratings for each child. Please
put down the figure that best indicates your evaluation of
the child's present thinking. Descriptive terms are also
given to help you in making your rating.
Any comments about the content or form of the ques-
tions, or about any difficulties that you may have in an-
swering them, will be welcomed.
Playfulness Rating Scale
I. A. How often does the child engage in spontaneous
physical movement and activity during play?
This behavior would include skipping, hopping,
jumping, and other rhythmic movements of the
whole body or parts of the body like arms, legs
or head, which could be judged as a fairly clear
indication of exhuberance.
Very
Often
Often
Occasionally
Rarely
Very
Rarely
B. How is his motor coordination during physical
activity?
Excellent
Very
Good
Good
Fair
Poor
II. A. How often does the child show joy in or during his
play activities?
This may be judged by facial expression such as
smiling, by verbal expressions such as saying "I
like this," or "This is fun," or by more indirect
vocalizing such as singing as an accompaniment of
the activity, f.i., "choo, choo, train, go along."
Other behavioral indicators would be repetition
of activity, or resumption of activity with clear
evidence of enjoyment.
Very
Often
Often
Occasionally
Rarely
Very
Rarely
2 1
Playfulness
Rating Scale
4 3
B. With what freedom of expression does he show his
joy?
This may be judged by the intensity of loudness of
a chuckle or a sing-song as well as the child's
ability to repeat or resume his activity by his
own choice.
Very
High
High
Moderate
Some
Little
III. A. How often does the child show a sense of humor
during play?
Very
Often
Often
Occasionally
Rarely
Very
Rarely
With what degree of consistency is humor shown?
Very
High High Moderate Some Little
IV. A. While playing, how often does the child show flex-
ibility in his interaction with the surrounding
group structure?
|
Full Text |
PAGE 1
IMAGINATIVE PLAY PREDISPOSITION, PLAYFULNESS, IDEATIONAL FLUENCY: THEIR RELATIONSHIPS IN KINDERGARTEN AND FIRST GRADE CHILDREN BY DEVORA SUE DEPPER A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADU J\ 'I' E COUNCIL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN P ARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA 1979
PAGE 2
Copyright 1979 By Devora Sue Depp er
PAGE 3
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This research would not have been possible without the support and guidance of many individuals. My doctoral com mittee provided both guidance in experimental rigor as well as personal support. Of particular note was my chair Jacquelin Goldman whose personal investment in my work has provided a significant enhancement of my professional development. In addition the gentle wisdom and loving support of Benjamen Barger has been unwavering throughout my graduate training. I would also like to thank the teachers and administration of P.K. Yonge Laboratory School. With out their cooperation this study would not have been pos sible. Finally, the immeasurable support and love of my family and friends enabled me to maintain my own playful ness throughout this research. iii
PAGE 4
TABLE OF CONTENTS Page ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ABSTRACT iii vi CHAPTER ONE TWO THREE FOUR INTRODUCTION AND HISTORICAL PERSPEC TIVE ON THEORIES OF PLAY Classical Theories Recent Theories .... IMAGINATIVE PLAY PREDISPOSITION, PLAY FULNESS, AND IDEATIONAL FLUENCY: THEORETICAL UNDERPINNINGS AND EXPERIMENTAL DATA BASE ......... Relationship of Play to Psychopathology Antecedents to Imaginative Play Relationship of Play Behavior and Playfulness Hypotheses METHODS OF PRESENT STUDY 1 2 5 16 19 20 25 32 35 Selection of the Population. 35 Subjects . . . . 36 Procedure . . . . . . 36 Means of Evaluation . . . 38 Validation and Reliability of the Tests............ 40 Data on Validity from the Literature 41 Data on Reliability from the Literature . . . . . . 41 The Playfulness Scale . . . . 42 The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test 43 Measuring Imaginative Play Predisposition . . . 44 RESULTS AND DISCUSSION Inter-rater Reliability Hypotheses I and II Hypothesis III .... Hypothesis IV .... Hypothesis V, VI and VII Discussion ...... iv 50 50 53 56 57 58 59
PAGE 5
APPENDICES A IDEATIONAL FLUENCY TASKS: INTERVIEW SCHEDULE . . . B IDEATIONAL FLUENCY TASKS: GUIDE FOR SCORING . . . C PLAYFULNESS SCALE . . D IMAGINATIVE PLAY PREDISPOSITION INTERVIEW . E HOLTZMAN SCORING REFERENCES BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH. . . . GUIDELINES V . . . Page 81 83 89 95 97 103 110
PAGE 6
Abstract of Dissertation Presented to the Graduat e Council of the University of Florida in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy IMAGINATIVE PLAY PREDISPOSITION, PLAYFULNESS, IDEATIONAL FLUENCY: THEIR RELATIONSHIPS IN KINDERGARTEN AND FIRST GRADE CHILDREN BY DEVORA SUE DEPPER JUNE 1979 Chairman: Jacquelin Goldman, Ph.D. Major Department: Psychology Children's play behavior has been the focus of numer ous experimental studies and theoretical conceptualizations. As a body of literature, however, it is markedly diverse and without agreement as to what play behavior is or its significance. This study examined three constructs: play fulness, imaginative play predisposition and ideational flu ency which have theoretically encompassed both the affective and cognitive functions of play behavior. This was done in an effort to provide a more unified theoretical conceptualization as well as an experimental data base for developmental aspects of play behavior. In addition, an extensive literature review of the theoretical literature on play behavior was provided. Seventy-nine kindergarten and first-grade white chil dren from five socioeconomic status backgrounds were stud ied in a semi-urban school. All children were of at least normal intelligence and participating in the regular school vi
PAGE 7
program. Playfulness, a multidimensional construct, was examined utilizing a teacher rating scale developed by Lieberman. This construct hypothesizes five qualitative and quantitative dimensions of playfulness: physical spon taneity, social spontaneity, humor, manifest joy, and cog nitive spontaneity. Imaginative play predisposition, the ability to interject an "as if" quality into play, was measured utilizing Singer's interview technique as well as the Holtzman Inkblot Test. Two different scoring tech niques were utilized for the Holtzman Inkblot Test in an effort to further clarify the methodolo g y necessary to measure imaginative play predisposition in a younger popu lation. Ideational fluency was measured utilizing Lieber man's modification of Torrance's work. It was hypothesized that these thr ee constructs oould be measured and repli cated on a sample of kindergarten and first grade children from heterogeneous socioeconomic status backgrounds in a semi-urban school. It was further hypothesized that there would be positive interrelationships between these behav iors, as measured, providing the beginning of the data base necessary for a more unitary theoretical conceptualization of play Imaginative play predisposition was replicated on this population with approximately 50 % of the subjects demon strating high imaginative play predisposition and 50 % dem onstrating low imaginative play predisposition. There were vii
PAGE 8
continued difficulties with the Holtzman Inkblot Test, as an appropriate methodology for a younger population. The evidence for ideational fluency was replicated on this pop ulation. The Playfulness Scale proved problematical in that the reliability of the data is somewhat questionable. Sig nificant relationships were found between imaginative play predisposition and cognitive spontaneity. The relationship between imaginative play predisposition as measured by the Singer interview plus the Holtzman movement score and the quantitative index for cognitive spontaneity as measured by the Lieberman rating scale was significant at the .005 level. Imaginative play predisposition as measured by the Singer interview plus the Holtzman movement score and the qualitative index for cognitive spontaneity as measured by the Lieberman rating scale was significant at the .05 level. Imaginative play predisposition as measured by the Singer interview plus the Holtzman movement plus human re sponse and the quantitative index for cognitive spontaneity as measured by the Lieberman rating scale was related at the .00 5 level of significance. Discussion provides clar ification of r e sults, new methodological considerations, as well as new dir e ctions for future research. viii
PAGE 9
CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION AND HISTORICAL PERSPECTIVE ON THEORIES OF PLAY Children's play has captured the interest of philos ophers, psychologists, anthropologists and sociolo g ists. As Plato is paraphrased by Erikson, the model of true play fulness is in th e need of all young creatures, animal and human, to leap. To truly leap, you must learn how to us e the ground as a springboard and how to land r esi liently and safely. It me a ns to test the leeway allowed by given limits; to out do and yet not escape g ravity. Thus, wherever playfulness prevails th e re is always a surpriz in g e l ement surpassing mere rep e tition or habituati o n and at its best sug ge stin g some virgin chance conquered. (Erikson, 1977, p. 17) Despite a longstanding interest, a variety of theoretical conceptualizations and numerous studies, there is little agreement as to what play is, or the significance of this be havior. Weisler and McCall (1976) note that there is an inflationary trend in the number of articles produced each year but a recession in the real production of knowledge in that area. They suggest that part of the difficulty li e s in the lack of a precise definition of play and the lack of a comprehensive theory of play. This paper will first provide a historical perspective on the various trends in play theory. Then two recent conceptualizations of play behavior, l
PAGE 10
2 Singer's (1968) construct of imaginative play and Lieber man's (1964) construct of playfulness will be discussed in light of their usefulness for providing unity in the area. A brief look at various theoretical perspectives of play is necessary at this point to provide a fuller per spective on psychological thinking regarding play behavior. There have been numerous conceptualizations of play. Gil more (1966) divides these theories into two categories. The classical theories, which are concerned with the ante cedents and purposes of play and not its content, comprise the first group. The more recent theories concerned with the form of play in relation to specifying its cause and effects comprise the second group. Classical Theories Within the classical theories, Gilmore (1966) notes that one of the oldest theoretical statements concerning the significance of play is attributed to both Schiller (1875) and Spencer (1873). This is the surplus energy theory. This theory states that because the young are taken care of by their parents, they hav e a surplus of energy, as they do not expend energy for self-preservation. This energy surplus is released through the exuberant activities of play. The theory postulates two things: first,
PAGE 11
3 there is a quantity of energy available to the organism; and second, that there is a tendency to expend this energy throu g h play, although it is not necessary for th e main tenance of a life balance (Gilmor e 1966). This theor y has been put forth by others. Gilmor e (1966) notes Terman (1932), Tinklepaugh (1942), and Al ex ander (1958). A second classical theory of play is the relaxation theory of play (Gilmore, 1966). Play activity is the prod uct of a deficit of energy, not a surplus. Lazarus (1883) and Patrick (1916) are associated with this th eo ry in which play is seen as the method by which spent e n e rgy is r ep len ished (Gilmore, 1966). Gilmore explains that play is a mode for dissipating the inhibition built u p from fatigue due to tasks that are new to the or g anism. It follows then that most play would occur in childhood, as this is a time for acquiring new skills. Play shows very little buildup of inhibition because it reflects deep rooted race habits (psychogenically acquired behaviors that are not new to the organism) (Gilmore, 19 66) Theorists have also seen play as a form of instinctive behavior (Gilmore, 1966). Britt and Janus (1941) and Beach (1945) list approximately two dozen theorists who see play as a form of instinctive behavior. Karl Groos (1 898 1908) is one of these theorists whose theory is known as the pre exercise theory of play. In this theory play is the product of eme rgin g instincts. Play is the exerc isin g of the
PAGE 12
4 emerging instincts in preparation for their time of matura tion. G. Stanley Hall (1906), a contemporary of Groo~ put forward his recapitulation theory of play. In this theory the purpose of play is to rid the organism of unnecessary instinctual skills which are the legacy of heredity. This is in striking contrast to Groos' theory; rather than de veloping new instinctual skills play is now proposed to eliminate primitive instinctual skills. Hall also postu lated stages of play. He was the first to do this, and stated that each child passes through stages corresponding to the cultural stages in the development of races (Gil more, 1966). Wundt (1913) was also a well-known recapitu lation theorist. An interesting approach to play with an anthropological bent came from Appleton (1910). Having contrasted play in primitive societies and children she suggested that play is a response to a generalized drive for growth in the or ganism. Rather than instinctual pre-exercise as suggested by Groos, play is the expression of hunger within the or ganism for growth to the stage at which the instinct could operate. Play, therefore, functions, as a facilitator for skills necessary to the function of adult instincts. Since the child wants mastery and "knows" that play is the method to achieve it, he plays. Gilmore (1966) labels Appleton's theory a growth theory of play.
PAGE 13
5 The early 1900's also produced the ego expanding the ories of play. K. Lange (1901) and Claraparede (1911, 1934) are considered to be the first proponents of this per spective (Gilmore, 1966). Ego in these theories equates with the reality mapping aspects of cognitive behavior. Claraparede saw play as an exercising of the ego that strengthened developing cognitive skills and facilitated the emergence of new skills. for ego completion. Lange saw play as the process Recent Theories The newer theories of play differ from the old in two respects. First is the focus on explanations of play based on dynamics of the individual personality, and the second is the concern with explaining individual changes in play behavior (Gilmore, 1966). Gilmore labels these the infan tile dynamics theories. Piagetian and psychoanalytic con cepts of play are the best known theories in this category. Lewin (1935) and White (1959, 1964) also fit in this cate gory. Lewin's position is not extensively elaborated. Play occurs because of the unstructured lifespace of the child. This results in a discrimination failure between what is real and what is unreal. It is easy, therefore, for the child to enter an unreal region where things are changeable and arbitrary.
PAGE 14
6 Piaget's theory of play is more comprehensive. Play is a result of the child's cognitive structure. For Piaget, play is the product of a stage of thinking through which the child must pass in order to develop from an original egocentric viewpoint to the adult's viewpoint (Gilmore, 1966). In order to understand Piaget's conceptualization of play it is necessary to examine the process of cognitive dynamics. Gilmore (1966) explicates this well. Every human behavior within his environment has two discriminable as pects which are central to Piaget 's theory. The first as pect is the organism recognizing, categorizing and utilizing events in terms of previous knowledge. He "bends reality" to conform to his habits, conventions and preferences The second is the individual's response to unique aspects of a new situation which he incorporat es to modify or to adjust to this "new reality." These two aspects are always present; one can, however, predominate over the other. Piaget suggests that these two aspects of behavior come from dif ferent sources, appear at different times and develop at different rates. It is this dynamic which leads to the appearance of play in children. Play is that behavior in which the aspect of adjusting reality to fit one's concept of reality predominates. The aspect of accomrro::lating to things as they really are takes a backseat. As this is an aspect of all behavior, al l behavior has some play -li ke aspects Behaviors are all more
PAGE 15
7 or less playlike, with respect to coping with reality. Play versus non-play behavior is not a relevant distinction. Piaget outlines three categories of play: practice play, symbolic play and "games with rules." Practice play is evidenced in the infant as the repetitious performance of any newly acquired ability. This will be performed in a variety of contexts. All new objects the infant encoun ters are made to fit this pattern regardless of their ap propriateness. New learning does not take place. In addi tion, the infant evidences pleasure with this behavior. Symbolic play has the characteristic of the child symboliz ing a behavior in a play. Gilmore (1966) cites the example of a child putting a rag to sleep. The ra g treated as though living, symbolizes to the child that which is salient to him in the concept of sleep. "Games with rules" develop later in life and the name is self-explanatory. Piaget also draws a parallel between play and dream ing. As in play, concepts in dreams are modified to fit existing emotions, often discounting obvious and logical parameters. Piaget also examined the development of play in the child. The newborn infant has only limited reflex abilities for processing his world cognitively, more specifically for recognizing and incorporating his experiences or to allow for uniqueness in his experiences. Postulated, however, is a tendency in all organisms to make repeated contact
PAGE 16
8 with a novel event This tendency "forces" in the infant new knowledge, change in habits and new distinctions regarding his environment The infant becomes able to act in a play -lik e manner as he becomes able to act by habit, thereby reducing the number of unique aspects of a given experience. The play potential is a g iven in the nature of the child and his cognitive structure. Play behavior is reduced in frequency as a function of the child's experiences. As the child has more expe riences, he learns more improved and rational modes of en countering the unfamiliar environment. The child no longer depends on partially appropriate (play-like) responses to new situations. Adult mastery is hallmarked by the indi vidual's reduced need to mold reality to fit his state of the moment. Gilmore summarizes Piaget's position as follows: Play is the behavior seen whenever there is a prepondernace of that aspect of all behavior that involves taking in, molding, and using things, all in terms of one's current inclina tion and habit, without deference to any as pects of so behaving that might not "fit" in some sense. Play can occur only insofar as be haviors are sophisticated enough to show dif ferenti a tion between the taking in aspect of behavior that bends reality to fit the self ; and the self modifying aspects of behavior that b e nd th e self to fit reality. Play can occur only insofar as there are many different modes of thought and action into which reality may be bent. Thus, it is that the newborn shows no play, and that until middle childhood more and more play is seen Finally, play will not occur insofar as more adaptive responses become
PAGE 17
9 familiar and can be easily in ve nted when needed. Thus, it is that play is reduced in prominence in late childhood. (Gilmore, 1966, p. 318) Cause and effects of play are sharpl y distinguished in Piaget's theory. There are two important products of play. The first is JOY or pleasure or some closely related state. Play brings with it the "functional pleasure of use" which is readily apparent in the infant engaged in practice play. The second product of play is adaptive. Play facilit a tes the retention of new abiliti e s. What may be lost throu g h disuse is maintained as these new abili ties get mor e attention than "reality." Piaget h a s also provided a syst em for categorizing types of play behavior. Briefly, h e has not ed two cate gories which reduce unpleasantness for the child. The first categor y is "compensatory combinations." This be havior "improves" reality through distortion to fit more agreeably with the child's conceptualization. The second ca te go ry is "liquidating combinations," in wh ich b e havior is freed from strong affect that initi a ted th e p la y b e hav ior. An example Piaget not e s is of his daughter. Af ter havin g been frightened by seeing a dead duck, the child p la yed at imitatin g the dead duck and made her dolls see a dead duck without fear. This source of play is similar to psychoanalytic conceptualization of play.
PAGE 18
10 A psychoanalytic concept of play was first introduced by Freud (1908, 1920, 1926) in regard to fantasy and repe' tition behaviors. Gilmore (1966) states that Freud thought of play as being closely related to fantasy behav ior; in fact he defined play as fantasy woven around real objects (toys) as contrasted with pure fantasy, which is daydreaming. Two classes of wishes were distinguished by Freud, either of which is necessary fo~ play. The first category consists of the wishes of a child to be grown up or in a more fortunate position. The child fantasizes a condition he wishes to exist, in accordance with his tendency to seek immediate pleasure. Secondly, play arises from the ten dency to repeat any experience which has been too much for him. In this the child wishes to take an active role in painful situations he experienced passively. Erikson clar ifies this as follows: Play often proves to be the infantile way of thinking over difficult experiences and of re storing a sense of mastery, comparable to the way in which we repeat, in ruminations and in endless talk, in daydreams and in dreams during sleep, expe riences which have been too much for us. (Erikson, 1959, p. 85) A "sense of mastery" is the most frequently cited effeet of play in psychoanalytic theory. This mastery feeling is limited to play which reverses a painful experience. Purely wish fulfillment play is pleasurable through the
PAGE 19
11 reduction of psychic tensions. Waelder (1933) pointed out that play can circumvent the action of the superego as well as reality. Play makes it possible to achieve what is physically or morally impossible. There have been some modifications of psychoanalytic theory of play notable by Anna Freud (1936) and Erik Erik son (1937, 1940, 1950, 1951, 1959). Anna Freud suggests that play may serve a defensive purpose as well as promot ing active coping behavior. In imitative play where the imitated object is feared, there is a lessening of the fear either of the object or what it represents. Erikison empha sizes the coping aspects of play (Gilmore, 1966). Erikson (1950) states, The human animal not only plays most and longest, it also remains ready to b e come deadly serious in the most irrational contexts. Gilmore (1966) notes that Erikson's concept of play disruption is perhaps his most important contribution to play theory. Anxiety leads to play but play can become stressful by mobilizing the anxiety it is trying to process. This results in an abrupt stop in play. Recently, Erikson (1977) has expanded play as a model for understanding the ritualization of human experi ence. In summary, psychoanalytic theory emphasizes the adap tive aspects of play behavior motivated by intrapsychic dynamics of the individual. Freud discussed play behavior within the framework of wish fulfillment. Here reality is
PAGE 20
12 modified to satisfy the drive to reduce conflict. A. Freud and Erikson further pointed out the adaptive aspects of mastery in play. Erikson (1950) states that there is mastery of reality through the creation of "model situations." White (1959) has addressed mastery derived through play be havior which is not based in conflict. White (1959) proposes that play behavior promotes a general relationship of effectiveness which the child seeks to maintain or to establish between himself and the environment. The control over animate and inanimate objects or situations, especially those which cannot be affected in reality, is the goal of "effectance." As the child matures, the gap between what others do and what the child accomplishes is reduced. Play is motivated by the ego competence energy which is a drive for effectiveness Play occurs because one feels inclined toward such behavior and finds it naturally satisfying (feeling of efficacy) White states Young animals and children do not explore be cause of a desire to practice useful skills and prepare for future contingencies. They p lay and explore because it is fun, because there is something inherently satisfying about it, not b e cause it is going to have a value in a future time. (White, 1964, p 34) Focusing on the more recent conceptualizations of play, Gilmore 's (1966) second category, play is conceptualized as serving several functions in the child. Cognitive theorists have emphasized the necessity for play in cognitive
PAGE 21
13 development. Psychoanalytic theorists have focused on its usefulness for intrapsychic development. White has suggested that the motivation for and utility of play is in a more general mastery over his world. Two recent the orists have also attempted to explicate the function of play behavior. Two new themes in the area of play are Singer's (1973) conceptualization of imaginative play and Lieberman's (1964) construct of playfulness. Both of th e se authors are addressing common aspects of play behavior. Each has articulated constructs which provide n ew information and clarity regarding more specific aspects of behavior within the general framework of play. By exam ining their similarities, it may be possible to provid e a mor e comprehensive understanding of play behavior. Singer (1973) defines imaginative play as the introduction of an "as if'' dimension to the individual's per ception of his experience. This is a modification of the environment based on experience carried in memory with early imagery. He notes that various other th eo rists (Freud, 1929; Lewin, 1935; Luria, 1932) hav e noted this transformation, pos tulatin g that this quality is fantasy in the child and daydreaming in th e adult. Singer further articulates this play behavior as play which requires a central generation of imagery and involves prete nd elements, i.e. changing of voices and roles, changes in time and
PAGE 22
14 space. This definition of play is consonant with m a ny previously postulated theoretical conceptualizations of play (psychoanalytic: A. Freud, 1937; S. Freud, 1958, 1959; Erikson, 1950; Hartmann, 1958; Piaget, 1962; White, 1959, 1960, 1964). Lieberman (1964, 1977) examines a core of personality traits which she labels playfulness which can be seen as an element in p lay, imagination, and creati v ity. She sug ge sts that there is a developmental continuity of playful ness as a behavior and that playfulness sur v ives play and becomes a personality trait of the individual. She notes that playfulness is made up of spontaneity, m a nifest joy and a sense of humor. Playfulness, then, can be se e n in the perspective of a qualitative aspect of play. Lieberman's work is also consonant with previous p la y theorists (Huizinga, 1955; Piaget, 1932; Whit e 1959, 1960, 1964). Each of these two theorists, Singer, and Lieberman, is examining play behavior in an effort to provide a the oretical unity incorporating the cognitive and affective dimensions of p lay. Singer (1966, 1973a) examines the play behavior with respect to the child and hypoth es izes the im p lications of this play behavior with respect to personality development and cognitive processes. Li e berm a n, on the other hand, explores the personality trait of p layfulness which is theoretically based around ima g ination and play behavior (Lieberm an 1977). By examining the relationship
PAGE 23
15 between these constructs and the cognitive component of ideational fluenc~ it is hoped that this author may provide further codification of our understanding of play behavior and generate further unity within a comprehensive theory of play.
PAGE 24
CHAPTER II IMAGINATIVE PLAY PREDISPOSITION, PLAYFULNESS, ANO IDEATIONAL FLUENCY: THEORETICAL UNDERPINNINGS AND EXPERIMENTAL DATA BASE The concept of imaginative play as defined by Singer (1973) provides a unity of various theories of play. Imaginative play is the ability to inject an "as if" or make-believe component in play. We can conceptualize this behavior as serving a variety of functions for the child. The use of imagination and fantasy provide the child with a tool to facilitate the organization of his world along both cognitive and affective dimensions. Psychoanalytic theorists have commented on the purpose of bending reality to meet the individual's needs. Cognitive theorists have articulated the process by which the child bends reality in order to organize his world cognitively. Freud discussed fantasy behavior within the framework of wish fulfillment. Here reality is modified to satisfy the drive to reduce conflict. A. Freud and Erikson pointed out the adaptive aspects of mastery in play involving imaginative elements. Erikson (1950) points out that there is a mastery of reality through the creation of "model situa tions" (imaginary situations). Hartmann (1958) stated that fantasy is an autonomous ego function which is developed without conflict. This expands the psychoanalytic 16
PAGE 25
17 conceptualization of play and fits well with Singer's con ceptualization. Singer's (1966) construct of imaginative play describes the process necessary to reduce conflict (Freud) or to gain mastery (A. Freud, Erikson). Modifica tion of reality by injecting an "as if" or make-believe com ponent enables the child to reduce conflict. Singer (1966) is defining the mechanism necessary to accomplish the func tion of play in psychoanalytic theory; that of bending reality to reduce conflict. Piaget's (1962) conceptualization of play as defined by the process of assimilation neatly supports Singer's theory. According to Piaget, play is not a behavior per se but a process. Play is the assimilation of reality to the ego. Play is distinguishable by a modif ic ation, varying in degree of the conditions of equilibrium between reality and the ego. The child utilizes assimilation in order to respond to his environment with the schemata available at a certain age. The introduction of make-believe elements aids in the maintenance of his perceptions at equilibrium or in balance with his world. Singer's (1966) construct of imaginative play is for the most part equivalent to Piaget 's (1962) construct of symbolic play. "Symbolic play, then, is only one form of thought linked to all others by its mechanism, but having as its sole aim satisfaction of the ego i.e., individual truth as opposed to collective and impersonal truth ... (Piaget, 1962, p. 167).
PAGE 26
18 Millar (1968) interprets aspects of play within a cognitive framework as well. She hypothesizes that imaginative play is an aspect of cognitive feedback processes which enable the human to code and process incoming data. This is also stated by Schachtel (1959). In examining the repetitive as pects of play, he notes that this repetition provides the opportunity to integrate new information into a limited ex perience background. White (1960) expands on this theme of cognitive mastery into other areas of behavior. White (1960) places the child's play behavior within the general developmental framework of striving for competence. The child utilizes play to expand his own capacities within the limits of his capacities. This includes social as well as, cognitive abilities. Tomkins (1962, 1963) ex pands the cognitive and affective components of play to note that it is utilized to organize the child's experience along both these dimensions. The peak of familiarity of material yields joy and laughter (Singer, 1966; Tomkins, 1962). Singer's (1966) construct of imaginative play effective ly provides a link between the affective and cognitive aspects of play behavior described in psychoanalytic and cognitive theories. It does so by providing th e mechanism by which the affective needs articulated in psychoanalytic th eory a re ac complished and by describing the same mechanism which cog nitive theorists have limited to information processing. As White (1960) expands the function of play behavior into
PAGE 27
19 the realm of competence, taking it beyond conflict reduc tion and cognitive processes, we can see that Singer's (1966) construct neatly provides the appropriate mechanism here as well. The child's affective development and cog nitive development are limited by his age, i.e., he has lim ited intrapsychic structure and cognitive schemata for pro cessing his world effectively. Imaginative play provides a mechanism for expanding his competence, given these lim itations, to achieve mastery. Relationship of Play to Psychopathology Singer expands his theoretical work to postulate as pects of personality development. Corrigan (1960) states that daydreaming can be useful as a coping mechanism. The development of imagination and daydreaming may be associ ated with a pattern of development and personality organiza tion. This personality organization is one in which think ing is valued and affective control and motor control are emphasized. Within this personality development Singer (1973) addresses the issues of pathological development. lie suggests that the high fantasy oriented individual will develop a more obsessive defense patt e rn. Th e l o w fantasy individual, on the other hand, will develop a more hyster ical style, where defenses are primarily repression and de nial.
PAGE 28
20 In essence Singer is proposing that those individuals who have a high imaginative play predisposition will then utilize this mechanism defensively to reduce conflict. Again it becomes clear that Singer is describing an inter nal process which describes one form of defense mechanism according to psychoanalytic theory. What he is proposing is that what psychoanalytic theory describes in children as play, wish fulfillment to reduce conflict, in adults is transformed into an internal obsessive defensive pattern. He goes one step further to p ropose that individuals with a high imaginative predisposition evid e nced in child hood as high frequency imaginative play behavior, are more likely to utilize this process defensivel y thereby develop ing a more obsessive personality style. Antecedents to Imaginati ve Play Singer's (1973) first study of imaginative play pre disposition explored several variables: background vari ab l es of the child and his relationship to his parents waiting behavior, creativity and personality characteris tics. Sin g er hypoth e sized that imaginativ e p lay vi e wed as a manif e station of co g nitiv e and affective s t y l e mi g ht well be associated with a pattern of develo p ment and personality organization in which thinking is valued and affective and motor control would b e more emphasized. Contact with at
PAGE 29
21 least one benign adult and the opportunity to be alone were seen as necessary to increased imaginative play development. In addition, from the perspective of psychopathology, high fantasizers were expected to utilize de fenses associated with obsessional characteristics, while low imaginative play children were expected to exhibit pat terns more like those of hysterical personalities with less self-consciousness and greater use of the mechanisms of de nial and repression. Singer examined forty children between the ages of 6 and 9 years, of middle SES backgrounds and of somewhat above average intelligence. The results supported several of the hypotheses and clearly pointed out differ ences between high and low imaginative play subjects. High imaginative play subjects reported greater associations be tween their parents and themselves, were able to wait for longer periods of time, had significantly higher ratings for creativity in a storytelling task and showed more ob sessional defense mechanisms than low imaginative play sub jects. Pulaski (1973) examined the effects of toy structure on imaginative play. It was hypothesized that minimally structured materials would stimulate more imaginative play than highly structured materials. Subjects in this study were kindergarten, first and second graders. Again this sample was homogeneous: upper middle class children attending a private school. The results were quite interesting.
PAGE 30
22 Minimally structured toys, for both low and high imaginative play subjects yielded a greater variety of themes in play than highly structured toys. High imaginative play chil dren played at a higher fantasy level than low imaginative play subjects. High imaginative play children also became more deeply absorbed in their play than did low imaginative play subjects. High imaginative play children also appeared to be more pleased and interested in their play than low imaginative play subjects. In addition, high imaginative play subjects showed a tendency to integrate more than one category of toy in their play. High imaginative play sub jects also showed greater flexibility in coping with demands upon their ability to produce fantasy. They also responded less negatively to interruptions in their play Overall, it can be seen that there was a distinct difference between low and high imaginative play children from equally priv ileged situations and with equally high intelligence. High imaginative play children were more original, creative, flexible and well integrated. Biblow (1973) examined drive reduction and mood change in the control of aggressive responses in high and low imaginative play subjects. Examining fifth grade white, middle class children of average intelligence, he found that high imaginative play subjects reduced aggression through the use of fantasy. Biblow concludes that the spectrum of his data supports that there are distinct differences between
PAGE 31
23 high and low imaginative play subjects, both in behavioral aggression and in all mood states. Freyburg (1973) examined the possibility of increas ing imaginative play in urban disadvantaged kindergarteners. Utilizing a public school, she found that after one month of training, the experimental group improved significantly in its use of imaginative play, as well as, in the expression of positive affect and increased concentration in play. The control group remained unchanged. The changes in the experimental group continued post -trainin g, and were tested during two months of post-training observations. Gott li eb (1973) also explored the modifiability of imaginative play. Noting the decrease of overt manifestations of make believe between the ages of 6 or 7 and 12, she chose to use two age groups as subjects: 10 to 12 and 12 1/2 to 14 year s. She was interested in exploring the difference in fantasy behavior at the two age levels, and its modifiability given a high or low fantasy predisposi tion. She found that expression of fantasy was increased for young children with exposure to an adult model. Junior high school age subjects' responses were more directly re lated to their imaginative play predisposition rather than modeling effects, i.e., they responded more in terms of their personality attributes She concluded that imagin ative behavior increases with age and as such it can be concluded that it is a skill that develops with
PAGE 32
24 differentiation, that there are age trends in ability to engage in fantasy and in the content of fantasy as well. The evidence for imaginative play predisposition sup ports that there are differences between low and high fan tasy predisposed individuals, that there are overt behavioral and personality differences. The behaviors related to fantasy appear to be modifiable given different environ ment variables. The above mentioned studies explore a variety of directions regarding imaginative play. It is important at this point to note, how if at all, they contribute to the theoretical basis of play behavior. Singer's (1973) study most directly explores theoretical concerns by addressing the defensive patterns utilized by the high and low imag inative play predisposed children. His finding that high imaginative play predisposed children utilize more obses sional defense mechanisms supports the hypothesis that this imaginative process may be related to the obsessional de fense mechanism, which in turn leads to the development of an obsessional personality style. The other issues he addresses regardin g parent-child relationshi p s, waiting be havior and creativity do not directly address theoretical issues. Rather they provide information regarding behavior to which imaginative play predisposition may be related. Pulaski's (1973) study focuses more on description of imaginative play in response to the stimulus of different
PAGE 33
25 toys. This study describes differential behavioral responses to play materials rather than directly providing support for theoretical questions. Biblow's (1973) study, in contrast to Pulaski's (1973) work, more directly explored theoretical issues. The cathartic theory of aggression from a psychoanalytic basis hypothesizes that fantasy operates to lower aggression. His data supported that hypothesis by establishing that high imaginative play subjects utilized fantasy to reduce aggression. This provides data regarding the utility of imaginative play as a mechanism to reduce conflict. Freyburg (1973) and Gottlieb's (1973) works both exam ine the modifiability of imaginative play. Again as with Pulaski's work, these studies provide information regarding the parameters of imaginative play but little direct support for theoretical conceptualization. Studies in imaginative play to date have served two useful and complimentary func tions: empirical support for certain aspects of theoret ical conceptualizations and description of the parameters of imaginative p lay. Relationship of Play Behavior and Playfuln e ss Singer (1973) and Lieberman (1977) have both addressed the tendency toward play in children. Sing e r (1973) sug ge sts that the tendency to engage in ima gi n at iv e p lay, fan tasied role shifting or daydreaming may be looked upon as a
PAGE 34
26 particular skill that can be developed in a given child as a consequence of the interaction between constitutional capacities with a particular set of early environmental cir cumstances that provide encouragement for practice. This tendency or trait in the individual has been addressed by Lieberman (1977) in her construct of playfulness. Lieberman (1964, 1977) examines a core of personality traits which she labels playfulness. Playfulness can be seen as an element in play, imagination and creativity. She postulates that there is a developmental continuity of playfulness as a behavior and that playfulness survives play and becomes a personality trait of the individual. She notes that playfulness is made up of spontaneity, manifest joy and a sense of humor. Playfulness can be seen in the perspective of a qualitative aspect of play. The theoretical underpinnings to Lieberman's (1964) work are less clear cut than Singer's work. Essentially, Lieberman's work attempts to address qualitative aspects of behavior rather than a discrete behavioral process that can be identified as evident or lacking. Playfulness is descriptive of the content of play behavior, as opposed to the structure of play, e.g., Singer (1973) imaginative play. Theoretically it is unclear what function playfulness serves. Playfulness is an expressive dimension of play behavior rather than a mechanism of play behavior.
PAGE 35
27 Qualitative aspects of play have received less atten tion in the psychological investigation of play. Huizinga (1955) refers to the play spirit. Joy, fun, pretend, and nonseriousness are key words in his conceptualization of play. While the fun element is seen as the part of play that resists logical analysis, it is at the same time regarded as characterizing the essence of play. Perhaps to the extent that play behavior is more or less playful, i.e., has more or less spontaneity, joy and humor in it, it is more closely related to the quintessence of play. How an individual child plays in relation to environ ment incorporates theoretically the playful component of play noted by Piaget (1932, 1962) and White (1960, 1964). This is the enjoyment or pleasurable component of play noted by Piaget (1962). White (1964) articulates this as fun, the inherent satisfaction of play behavior. This is a key aspect of how the clinician looks at play. It is the deviation from the play spirit which is important. Moustakas (1955) reports on play therapy with normal chil dren. He notes that these children are happy, often singing and humming, and in their actions they are both more deci sive and spontaneous than disturbed childr e n. Hartley, Frank, and Gold e nson (1952) found in nurser y -school children that well-adjusted children played as enthusiastically as the troubled youngsters, but their delight in toys was g reater.
PAGE 36
28 Developmental psychologists h ave also referred to the qualitative aspects of play. For example, Piaget (1962) distinguishes playful from imitative behavior and describes it as a process whereby the child incor porates external ob jects into his own thought schemata in a joyful manner. Hunt (1961) finds function pleasure in play as a q uality that frequently accompanies learnin g as a result of aimless activity. Playfulness emerged from observational studies of how children play (Lieberman, 1977). In an effort to examine theoreticall y complex behavior she defined operationally playfulness as physical, social, and cognitiv e spontaneity, manifest JOY and sense of humor. Within the framework of p lay, it was seen a s a quality of play These operational definitions of the qualitative aspects of p lay represent the first step in prov iding an adequate empirical investiga tion of p la yf ulness. It is readily apparent that the th eore tical underpinnings for the construct are mor e so p histicated than the operational definiti on s based on loose l y defined behavioral observations. The p l ayf ulness con struct is an ambi tious attempt to operat i ona lize the ex pre ssiv e content of p lay b e havior. Bot h psychoa n a l ytic theory and co g nitive th eo ri es hav e mor e directly addressed the functional and structural aspects of play behavior while alluding to the qualitative or expre ssiv e dimensions of this behavi or What Li eberma n (1964) is at t empting to
PAGE 37
29 provide is a perspective of the content of play behavior: social, cognitive and affective. In light of this, how ever, this is the best methodological exploration to emerge out of the theoretical speculation. Lieberman (1964, 1977) has examined playfulness at two age levels: kindergarteners and adolescents. The research on kindergarteners investigated the relationship be tween playfulness and divergent thinking. Utilizing mid dle-class children attending private kindergartens, she found that there was a unitary trait called playfulness composed of five dimensions: physical, cognitive, and social spontaneity, manifest joy and a sense of humor. The relationship b e tween divergent thinking and playfulness was not so clear. But a clear r e lationshi p betwe e n pla yf ulness and ideational fluency and spontaneous flexibility (two as pects of divergent thinking) was found. The strongest re lationship was between ideational fluency and playfulness. The adolescent study looked at high school students, noting that there is a resurgence of the joyful-spontaneity sense of humor syndrome during adolescence a nd that this be havior syndrom e is related to cognitiv e st y l e (Lieberman, 1977). Preliminary results yield a more compl ex picture for adolescents than kinder ga rten ers It is not clear whether or not there is a "pure" playfulness factor for adolescents. The research was conducted in a classroom be havior of int e llectu a l commitment which is not c o nsonant
PAGE 38
3 0 with the bubbling effervescence of playfulness. Some pre liminary work is currently being done on a college age population. From the work of Singer (1973) and others (Biblow, 1973; Freyburg, 1973; Gottlieb, 1973; Pulaski, 1973) there appears to be evidence for high and low imaginative play predisposed individuals. In addition, the work of Lieberman (1964) provides evidence for a playfulness trait in children. In order to further describe these behavioral traits it is necessary to expand the existing data base, i.e., to replicate the findings on a broader data base as well as explore the relationships between these traits. The present study explores the relationships between imaginative play, playfulness, and ideational fluency. Ideational fluency was chosen as an effort to replicate Lieberman's (1964) findings and to provide further data as to the structure of the cognitive aspect of imaginative play. Qualities frequently noted in children's play are its imaginative scope and creative power (Lieberman, 1964). There has been, however, little research done on young chil dren in regard to this creativity. In their initial work on creative thinking in adults, Guilford et al. (1951) hypoth esized eight abilities that might "discover the individual who is potentially creative." Among these eight was ideational fluency. This is the calling up of ideas in
PAGE 39
3 1 situations demanding relatively little restriction, the difference in scores being more a function of the number of ideas produced than the quality, degree of appropriate ness, or the aptness of expression of the ideas (Guilford, et al., 1956). While Guilford was refining his factors looking at adults, Torrance (1960) and his associates used his con ceptual framework and an adaptation of his tests to examine creative thinking in children. The Minnesota research team was able to develop batteries of nonverbal and verbal creative thinking tasks. One of the constructs which could be meaningfully scored was ideational fluency. The purpose behind these studies was to provide a description of and a means of testing creative potential. The present study grew out of several considerations. There exists extensive and sophisticated theoretical formu lations regarding play behavior. The empirical support for these theories is wide ranging, divergent, and methodolog ically poorly developed. Rather than develop new constructs and instruments to measure them, it seemed more useful to work with currently defined constructs which overlapped theoretically. Relationships between playfulness and imag inative play predisposition would provide information re garding both the structure and the content of play behavior. While recognizing the methodologies as first steps in oper ationalizing complex phenomena working with these
PAGE 40
32 methodologies to explore their utility is an appropriate investigation. If in fact the constructs address similar aspects of behavior, as their theoretical bases sug ge st, then establishing empirical relationships could provide new directions for empirical and theoretical work in play. The evidence for playfulness, imaginativ e play predispo sition and ideational fluency has been limited to homogen eous samples: middle class urban children. In an effort to expand this data base it seemed d e sirable to replicate the data on a less homogeneous sample. Hypotheses Hypotheses 1 and 2 Based on both psychoanalytic and cognitive theoretical conceptualizations of play behavior, Sin ger (1973) has op erationally defined imaginative play behavior as an aspect of play behavior which encompasses the ability to inject an "as if" component into play. Utilizing the methodology de veloped by Sin ger (1973), as well as the Holtzman Inkblot Test It is hypothesized that the evidence for high and low imaginative play predisposed individuals can be replicated o n an het e ro geneo us SES sam p le of kindergarten and first grade children. It is further hypothesiz ed that a more app ropriate scoring technique for the Holtzman Inkblot Test utilizes Human plus Movement responses.
PAGE 41
33 Hypothesis 3 Lieberman (1964) has operationally defined the con struct playfulness to address the qualitative aspects of play behavior based on observations of children's behavior. Utilizing the methodology developed by Li ebe rman (1964), it is hypothesized that the evidence for a playfulness as pect of behavior can be replicated on a SES heterogeneous sample of kindergarten and first grade children. Hypothesis 4 Ideational Fluency, developed by Guilford (1951) as an aspect of creative thinking in adults, was expanded on by Torrance (1960) to explore creative thinkin g in chil dren. Utilizing Lieberman's (1964) methodolo gy for kinder ga rten children, it is hypothesized that th e ev idence for ideational fluency can be replicated on an heterogeneous SES sample of kindergarten and first grade children. Hypotheses 5 and 6 Given the theoretical overlap of imaginative play pre disposition and playfulness it is hypothesized that these two constructs are highly related in kindergarten and first grade children. High imaginative play predisposed children likewise should demonstrate behavior rich in the quality dimension of playfulness. It is further hypothesized that the subcomponent of cognitive spontaneity in playfulness will have the greatest relationship to imaginative play pre disposition, as it most directly examines imagination.
PAGE 42
34 Hypothesis 7 In addition, it is hypothesized that ideational flu ency is related to both playfulness and imaginative play predisposition in this sample of kindergarten and first grade children.
PAGE 43
CHAPTER III METHODS OF PRESENT STUDY Selection of the Population The kindergarten-first grade classes of the P.K. Yonge Laboratory School of the University of Florida were selected for the study. Due to its relationship with the University of Florida the students are accustomed to a variety of in terruptions in their normal school day whether it be for experimental purposes or special observers or visitors It was felt, therefore, that there would be fewer effects in troduced by the experimental interv e ntion per seas this is seen as "normal" in this school. In addition, as the school selects its population to reflect fiv e SES levels of the community and a heterogeneous population was desirable, the population was ideal. There were four combined kindergarten-first grade classes. Arrangements were made with each of the four teachers to gai n her cooperation and interest in the study. Essentially all the Caucasian students were utiliz ed to prov ide a balanced sample for se x and SES l ev e l. 35
PAGE 44
36 Subjects Eighty kindergarten and first grade children attending the P.K. Yonge Laboratory School of the University of Flor ida were selected. There were 40 females and 40 males. One male child moved during the data collection period re sulting in a total sample of 79, 40 female and 39 male. All subjects were Caucasian and participating in the regular class program. The population attending P.K. Yonge School is drawn equally from five SES levels in a semi-urban population. The students are selected for P.I. Yonge Laboratory School to reflect both the ethnic and SES composition of the com munity. The five SES levels are as follows: Level I in come-< $6,000, Level II--$6,000 $10,499, Level III$10,500 $14,499, Level IV--$15,000 $20,999, Level v$21,000+. All subjects were of at least normal intelligence as measured by the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. The IQ range was from 91 162, with the mean IQ 113. Procedure Thursday or Friday of the week prior to the data collec tion the experimenter visited the classroom for the day This enabled the experimenters to become more familiar with the classes' daily routine and physical layout. The experiment ers consisted of two senior undergraduate psychology majors
PAGE 45
37 in addition to the primary investigator. The primary investigator collected data from two classrooms, the other experimenters each collected data from one classroom. Each of the four classrooms had one experimenter for the period of one week. Each subject accompanied the ex perimenter to a small workroom adjacent to the classroom for three separate testing sessions. Each session ranged from 15-30 minutes. Each of the three sessions comprised one aspect of the evaluation. The experimental condition was broken down into three sessions to accorr~od.ate the child's attention span. Session A consisted of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; Session B consisted of the ideational fluency tasks and the Imaginativ e P lay Predis position Interview; Session C consisted of the Holtzman Inkblot Test. The experimental tasks were presented to the subject in a randomized sequence to account for order ef fects. All of the subjects in one classroom were processed in one week, except for circumstances of school absence. No two sets of evaluation instruments were administered in one day. Concurrent with th e experimenter's data collection from the students was data co llection from th e t eac hers. Initially the primary investigator met with th e teachers to discuss the Playfulness Rating Scales, to explain the instructions and to answer any questions. A copy of the rating sc a l e was left with the teach e rs. They were asked
PAGE 46
38 to keep the rating scale in mind as they observed their students for 2 weeks. A second session with the experi menter was held to further discuss th e ratin g scale and to answer questions. The teachers were th e n asked to rate the children. Means of Evaluation Evaluation instruments for this study covered three defined areas of inquiry: ideational fluency, playfulness and imaginative play predisposition. In addition, a standardized p ictur e vocabulary test was used to estimate in telligence. Ideational fluency was examined utilizing two tasks: (a) Product Improvement Task, and (b) The Monroe Language Classification Test. A description of the tests is as fol lows. The Product Improvement Task is a two part test. For Part I the subject was presented with a stuffed dog made of brown plush material, approximately 7 inches long. The dog had button eyes, a button black nose, a red tongue and a red ribbon around its neck. The following instructions were given : Well what hav e w e her e It is a toy dog Now you tell me the most int e r e sting, clev erest ways by which you can change this dog (or: make him different) so that you and other children would have more fun playing with it. You just tell me how and I'll write down your ideas.
PAGE 47
39 The subject's responses were recorded for l minute JO sec onds. For Part II the subj ect was presented with a cloth doll, McCalls Pattern #5724. The doll was approximately 11 inches long with brown yarn hair and a blue denim shirt and pants. The following instructions were given : What else do we have here? It's a doll. Now you go ahead and tell me the most interesting and cleverest ways by which you can change this doll so that it will be more fun to play with. The subject's responses were recorded for l minute JO sec onds. See Appendix A for the tasks. The Monroe Language Classification Test was a three part test. The subject was given the following instructions: No w this is the last thing we 'r e going to do ." "Tell me all the animals you can think of as quickly as you can." "Tell me all the things to eat you can think of." "Tell me all the toys you can think of ." Responses for each category were recorded in al minute 30 second time block. See Appendix A for the tasks. The Product Improvement task and the Monroe Language Classification Test were developed and/or adapted by Lieber man (1964) in her work on divergent thinking in kindergarten children. Ideational fluency, an as pec t of div ergent think ing, was selected to be examined in this study a s it yielded the results of greatest significanc e in her work. Lieber man (1964) utilized the following crit e ria for development of her instrum ents : (a) applicability of existing tests,
PAGE 48
40 or of specially constructed tests modeled after existing tests, at the kindergarten level; (b) evidence for validity and reliability of these tests from a pilot study for her initial research project and from the existing literature. Based on the work by Guilford (1951) and Torrance (1960) on creative thinking and Gewirtz (1948) and Meyers (1962) on verbal fluency in young children, Lieberman (1964) developed the following tasks: Product Improvement, Guilford's Plot Titles, and the Monroe Language Classification Test. From the Monroe Test the objects selected were Animals, Things to Eat, and Toys. The child was asked to list as many as he could think of. The tasks are included in Appendix A. Validation and Reliability of the Tests Lieberman's (1964) final selection of the tests was made after a pilot study with 14 subjects attending a pri vate kindergarten in New York City. The age range was from 4-6 to 6-3 years, the average being 5-0 years. Meaningful scores were obtained for ideational fluency from all three tests. She noted that teacher conferences provided a pre liminary validity check since the children who scored high on the divergent thinking factors were jud ged creative by the teachers. Lieberman developed a special scoring guide for the ideational fluency tasks (1964). (See Appendix B.) As far
PAGE 49
41 as possible, the rationale was mod e led after existing tests by Torrance and Guilford. The introduction of the new prod uct, namely the doll and the original text of the stories, called for specific guidelines for the answers obtained. Interscorer reliability for the div e rgent thinking scores was established in the Lieberman study on a sam p le of 20 records. The Pearson r's were .99 for ideational f luency on all three tasks. Data o n Validity from th e Lit e ratur e The evidence about the tasks measurin g id ea tional fluency is as follows. (a) For Product Impro ve m e nt Torrance (1960) claim ed face validity and gave scorin g categories on the basis of a sample of 146 elementary school children, grades 1-6. (b) Analysis of th e Monroe Lan g u age Classifi cation Test showed a factor loading of ideational fluenc y (Meyers et al., 1962) and comparable tests of object naming also showed loadin g s on ideational fluency (Bereiter, 1961; Guilford & Christensen, 1956). Data on Reliability from th e Literature For the Prod uct Improv e m e nt Task Torrance (1 960 ) men tioned the int e rchan ge abilit y of a to y m o nk ey with the toy dog and cited intersc ore r r e liabilit y in th e .80's, h ence the s e l ec tion of a dog in Lieberman (1964). Meyer s et al.
PAGE 50
42 (1962) reported a reliability of .62 based on intercorrela tions of the three parts for the Monroe Language Classifi cation Test (Animals, Toys, Things to Eat). The Playfulness Scale Playfulness was evaluated utilizing the Playfulness Scale developed by Lieberman (1964). (See Appendix c.) This scale consists of five subscales corresponding to the five behavior traits of physical spontaneity, manifest joy, sense of humor, social spontaneity, and cognitive spontane ity. Each scale is divided into an A and B part, referring to the quantity and quality of the trait measured. These subdivisions were made in order not to contaminate frequency with degr ee or intensity of the trait m eas ured. It was found that the further refinement was considered helpful to the raters (Lieberman, 1962). The division also indicated no prior assumption that quantity and quality were related. Ratings are made on a five point scale. Descriptiv e labels for the points on the scale are given, as are sample behav ior items for each trait to be rated. The format of th e scale was modeled after Beller's (1955) instrum e nt for as sessing dependence, independ e nce and aggression in young children through teacher ratings (Lieberman, 1964). As a check on validity, Lieberman (1964) included two questions not related to the behavior indices for playfulness. These
PAGE 51
43 scales asked for an evaluation of the child's intelligence and his physical attractiveness. Reliability coefficients, obtained from the ratings of the two teachers for the five traits and considered quali tatively and quantitatively, ranged with a Spearman-Brown correction from .66 to .86 with a mean of .70 (Lieberman, 1964). Intercorrelations between the playfulness traits ranged from .61 between sense of humor and physical spon taneity to .86 between manifest joy and cognitive spontane ity (Lieberman, 1964). When centroid factors were ex tracted, four of the five playfulness traits (cognitive spontaneity, social spontaneity, manifest joy, sense of hu mor) had loadings in the middle .80's on the first factor. Physical spontaneity had a loading of .78. Factors 2 and 3 accounted for little of the variance with factor loadings ranging from .092 to .379. Lieberman (1964) concluded from this that Playfulness could be conceptualized as a unitary trait. The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test This test is an individual intelligence test for ages 2 1/2 to 18 years. It is designed to provide a well stan dardized estimate of a subject's verbal intelligence (Dunn, 1965). This test was chosen for its ease of administration and previously noted appropriateness (Lieberman, 1964). As an instrument using verbal stimuli and nonverbal responses,
PAGE 52
44 it aims at an assessment of verbal comprehension and learned information. It is, therefore, considered sufficiently dif ferent to provide some control measure for the scores on the ideational fluency tasks (Lieberman, 1964). Also testing time is short, which is an important consideration for use with kindergarteners. The test consists of 150 plates. Each plate has four well drawn and unambiguous pictures from which the subject is asked to choose one response to the examiner's question. The subject need only point. The score is the number of correct answers. The manual suggests starting points for different age levels. With respect to the difficulties experienced with mea sures of intelligence below age 6, the evidence cited for validity and reliability can be considered satisfactory. A correlation of .79 with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children has been obtained for a group of above average elementary school children. Reliability coefficients are given by age level and are .73 and .67 for ages 5 and 6, respectively (Dunn, 1965). Measuring Imaginative Play Predisposition Singer (1973) notes that a major effort aimed at ar ticulating more precisely what goes into imaginative tenden cies in children and adults has grown out of the use of projective techniques. Projective techniques utilize the
PAGE 53
45 the presentation of relatively ambiguous stimuli to indi viduals who are then required to tell stories about pic tures or to give associations to abstract inkblots, etc. These ambiguous testing instruments were developed in an effort to provide behavioral assessment of underlying inclinations. Singer (1968) views projective techniques as a method to tap ongoing behavioral tendencies in the individual. The use of projective methods for studying imagination has generally involved what the content reveals about spe cific conflict areas, as can be seen from the widespread use of the Thematic Apperception Test or various forms of association tests (Singer, 1973). A different approach has developed usin g the Rorschach Inkblot Method. Rorschach (1942) provided a major insight by noting that the tendency to produce movement or color responses when looking at the inkblots tapped not so much the specific content of con flicts or needs, but rather measured a broad trend toward reliance on imagination or an open expression of emotionality as a major tendency (Singer, 1973). Rorschach proposed that all human experience could be measured along an intro version-extratension dimension and that the ratio of the human movement (M) responses in inkblots to the color re sponses in producing associations was a fundamental way of tapping this predisposition (Rorschach, 1942). On the whole, there seems to be considerable support in research for at least some aspects of Rorschach's interpretation of the
PAGE 54
46 human movement responses as a measure of imaginativeness in both children and adults (Singer, 1960, 1968, 1973). Singer (1973) links the development of the human movement (M) re sponse to the internalization of speech in the form of heightened imagery and fantasy. This provides th e basis for the choice of the inkblot method as one approach to estimat ing imaginative predisposition to play. Normative data with the original set of Rorschach inkblots indicate that chil dren show relatively few such movement responses before the age of 6 or 7 (Ames et al., 1974). It was necessary, then, to find another technique appropriate to a younger popula tion. The Holtzman Inkblot Test (Holtzman, 1961), however, provides useful norms on performance of children as young as 5 years. The data from the Holtzman Inkblots strongly support the importance of the movement response as an index of ideational tendencies (Singer, 1973). The Holtzman Inkblots were developed in an attempt to provide a projective technique with psychometrically sound scoring procedures for responses to inkblots while also pre serving the rich qualitative projective material of the Rorschach (Holtzman et al., 1961). The Holtzman Inkblots consist of 47 cards (45 are utilized in the score, 2 are practice cards). The subject is required to provide one response to each card. The cards are then scored, utilizing a standard format. Various studies have examined the com parability of the Holtzman Inkblot responses and the
PAGE 55
47 Rorschach Inkblot responses. Most of the recognized Rorschach responses can be reproduced by configural scoring utilizing the basic elements coded in the Holtzman technique (Holtzman et al., 1961). Haggard, cited in Holtzman (Holtz man et al., 1961), utilizing multivariate analysis, found significant correlations across eight scores in the responses of high school students. Important to note is that the move ment response was one of the eight scores examined. Holtz man et al. (1961) conclude that these results indicate quite conclusively that the Rorshach and Holtzman systems have a great deal in common as far as the underlying meaning of their respective variables is concerned. Holtzman et al. (1961) provide an equivalency table for converting movement (M) responses according to the Klopf e r method of Rorschach scores as Holtzman Inkblot scores of M > l plus H > l. Biblow (1973) utilized the Holtzman Inkblots to deter mine imaginative predisposition with fifth grade children. Utilizing the blots, fantasy level was determined upon the basis of the score for movement responses to 20 selected cards. Gottlieb (1973) also utilized the movement (M) score on one-half of the Holtzman Inkblots, Form A, with elemen tary school and junior high school age children, as an as pect of imaginative predisposition evaluation. The movement (M) response on the Holtzman blots is scored for inanimate, as well as animate, movement. Rorschach (1942), however, stated that movement (M)
PAGE 56
48 responses applied only to human or animal (animate) movement. In an effort to examine the introversive elements articulated by Rorschach as related to imagination in the individual, this study examines different combinations of scores of the Holtzman blots: M scores alone and M>l plus H > l. The odd-numbered cards, Form A, of the Holtzman Ink blots were used in this study. Odd-even reliability coef ficients for the procedure were suffici ent to utilize one half of the instrument to allow for the kindergarteners' limited attention span. The correlation coefficients for each of the responses for the 5 year old population are as follows: Movement (M) .80, Human (H) .79. Singer has developed an interview for assessing imaginative play predisposition (Singer, 1973). (See Appendix D.) He has noted that it is possible to get adequate data from children as young as kindergarten age. The questions are as follows: (a) What is your favorite game? What do you like to play the most? (b) What game do you like to play best when you are all alone? What do you like to do best when you are all alone? (c) Do you ever have pictures in your head? Do you ever see make-believe things with pic tures in your mind or think about th e m? What sort of things? (d) Do you have a make-believe friend? Do you have an animal or toy or make-believe person you talk to
PAGE 57
49 or take along with you? Did you ever have one, even though you don't any more? The key in scoring these items has to do with the de gree to which the child's report indicates the introduction of symbolic play or make-believe. The questionnaire was scored on a five point scale (range O 4 positive imagin ative responses). Singer (1973) notes that most children do not receive a score higher than 2 or 3. He concludes that scores of O l indicate low imaginative p lay predis p osition, while scores of 2 or more indicate high i mag ina tive play predisposition.
PAGE 58
CHAPTER IV RESULTS AND DISCUSSION Inter-rater Reliability The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test was scored accord ing to manual instructions, and the subjects received an IQ score. The Monroe Language Classification Test was scored blind by three raters. Two raters were senior undergrad uate psychology majors; one rater was a first year graduate student in clinical psychology. The raters participated in a training session with the primary investigator. The rat ers then independently rated all the protocols. Inter-rater reliability was established utilizing a Spearman Correla tion Coefficient. For the Monroe Language Classification Test inter-rater reliability for the three raters was as follows: r= .99, r= .97, r= .97. The Imaginative Play Predisposition Interview responses were scored blind following the same procedur e s as described for the Monroe Language Classification Test. Inter-rater reliability was established utilizin g th e Spearman Correlation Coefficient. Inter-rater reliability for the Imaginative Play Predisposition Interview was as follows: r = .80, r = .83, r = .82. 50
PAGE 59
51 The Product Improvement Task was rated by two senior undergraduate psychology majors. The procedure for rating was described above for the Monroe Language Classification Test. Inter-rater reliability was established utilizing the Spearman Correlation Coefficient. The inter-rater re liability was= .96. The Holtzman Inkblot Tests were scored by three ad vanced graduate students in clinical psychology who had ex perience in administering and scoring projective tests and one less advanced graduate student in clinical psychology. Each rater pa rticipated in a trainin g session with the pri mary investigator to learn how to score the Holtzman blots for human and movement responses. Each rater then scored approximately 20 protocols (one rater scored 19). The pri mary investigator served as a standard with which th e raters' scores were compared for inter-rater reliability. Four of the protocols were randomly selected for comparison as follows. Each rater scored five protocols and one was randomly selected for comparison prior to completing the remaining protocols. The standard then ind epende ntly rated the target protocol. Differences in s co rin g were discussed but the scorin g was unchanged. This proced ur e was followed until all the protocols w e r e complet e d. A Tau B w as utilized for c a lculating inter-rater reliabilit y. ities were as follows: Reliabil
PAGE 60
52 Rater l Human Responses T = .94 Movement Responses T = .86 Rater 2 Human Responses T = 83 Movement Responses T = 9 9 Rater 3 Human Responses T = 72 Movement Responses T = .90 Rater 4 Human Responses T = .88 Movement Responses T = .93 It was hypothesized that there would be significant re lationships between imaginative play predisposition and playfulness, with the relationship between the subitem cog nitive spontaneity and imaginative play predisposition as most notable. In fact, the only relationships of significance generated by the regression equations were between cognitive spontaneity and imaginati ve play predisposition. Imaginative play predisposition measured by the Singer in terview plus Holtzman movement score was significant at the .005 level with the quantitative index for cognitive spontaneity. The slope of the regression was negative. Imaginative play predisposition as measured by the Singer inter view plus Holtzman movement response was significant at the .05 level with the qualitative index for cognitive spontaneity. The slop e of th e regr e ssion was positive. Im ag inative p lay p redis positi on as measured by th e Sin ger intervi e w p lus the Holtzman movement plus human res p onse was significantly related to the quantitative index for playfulness. The slope of th e regression was negative.
PAGE 61
53 There was no evidence for a significant relationship between ideational fluency and playfulness. Hypotheses I and II Measuring Imaginative Play Predisposition With respect to the interview task designed by Singer, he suggests that frequency of high (2 or greater) and low (0 1) imaginative play predisposition would be evenly divided in a normal population. In this study the sample was divided approximately in half (38 high, 49 low) with the high imaginative play predisposition defined as a score of 2 or greater and the low imaginative play predisposition defined as a score of O or 1. (See Table l for distribution of scores.) Imaginative play predisposition was related to sex in this sample. Females scored higher than males (F<.01). Another question raised by this study was the appropriate scoring technique for the Holtzman Inkblot Test. Spearman Correlation Coefficient for the relationship between the movement responses alone and the combination human plus movement responses was r = .78, suggesting that at this age the respons es are essentially the same. Th e relationship be tween the imaginative play predisposition and the Holtzman movement responses was= .14 and the imaginative play pre disposition and Holtzman human plus movement responses
PAGE 62
54 = .22; neither relationship was significant. This lack of correlation suggests that the Imaginative Play Predis position Interview and the Holtzman Inkblot Test may still be aspects of the complex phenomenon of imaginative play predisposition but are clearly not measuring the same com ponents. For the purposes of this study both types of scor ing were examined in a linear regression model to examine relationships between the Holtzman Inkblot score and the other variables. Both movement and movement plus human responses were positively related to age: imaginative play predisposition, movement alone: F < .01; imaginative play predisposition, movement plus human response: F < .005. This would suggest that the older the child the higher the score on the Holtzman task. This would confirm developmental trends noted by Holtzman (cited in Hill, 1972). Others have suggested (Ames et al., 1974) that this developmental trend is evident in Rorschach movement (M) responses as well. This introspective aspect of development does perhaps develop over time. When the interview score and the Holtzman Ink blot score were combined it also reflected the relationship to age: imaginative play predisposition, Holtzman movement along/age: F~.005; imaginative play predisposition, Holtz man movement plus human response/ag e : F<.001. This may suggest that the age effect in the Holtzman score is so strong that the combined imaginative play predisposition is age related. Imaginative Play Predisposition scores without
PAGE 63
55 the Holtzman component were not age related. Singer (1966), however, suggests that this trait is not age related but rather is a quantifiable entity within an individual, which may show development over time but should not directly be affected by age. This raises some interesting questions. If indeed, as Singer suggests, there is an imaginative play predisposition and it can be measured by his interview task without age demonstrating a clear relationship then the utility of including the Holtzman measure with its strong relationship to age is questionable. It would seem that adding this measure, although hypothesized theoretically as a component of imaginative play predisposition, is so af fected by developmental trends as to perhaps eliminate or confound the true imaginative play predisposition. On the other hand one could question whether or not this is an artifact of the age range examined. The frequency of movement (M) responses shows more marked change in these years than at other ages. In Holtzman's normative data cited in Hill (1972), 5 year olds at the 48th percentile generated a raw score of 9; first graders at the 50th percentile generated a raw score of 14. Norms on fourth graders reflected a raw score of 24 at the 50th percentile ~hich was equiva lent to percentile norms for the average adult population This could indicate that the Holtzman Inkblot Test for a younger population is too heavily affected by developmental trends to serve as an adequate measure of imaginative play
PAGE 64
56 predisposition for these early years. The Holtzman task, however, was selected as it seemed more appropriate than a Rorschach at these early years. A more suitable method of examining imaginative play predisposition needs to be de veloped for these early years. Hypothesis III Measuring Playfulness Lieberman's (1964) scale measures five separate aspects of playfulness on both a quality and quantity dimension. Each subdivision--physical spontaneity, manifest joy, social spontaneity, cognitive spontaneity--can receive a rating from 1 to 5 for each (quality, quantity). A child's com bined quality of playfulness score, therefore, has the range of 5 25. A child's combined quantity of playfulness score has the range of 10 50 (quality+ quantity). In this sample the playfulness scores did not distribute themselves adequately over the range to reflect both high and low play fulness scores. Over 50% (n=58) received scores of 36 or g reater. As can be seen in Table 3 the sample reflected al most no children with low playfulness scores. When the scores were broken down by SES levels (see Table 4) it can be seen that this trend was evident in all SES levels. The finding of high playfulness scores raises some in teresting questions. Utilizing Lieberman's instrum e nt it is evident that Lieberman's (1964) results were replicated.
PAGE 65
57 As Lieberman's (1964) sample also reflected a high playful ness bias. The teachers were able, in rating the children, to utilize all five points on each of the subdivisions (some students did receive scores less than 3); howeve~ the ma jority of students received scores of 3 or greater on all the subdivisions. Two of the teachers were able to utilize all five points on the Lieberman scale. The remaining two teachers utilized four out of the five points on the Lieber man scale. Tables 5-8 illustrate the four teachers' ratings. The playfulness rating scale requir e s further examination on more sampl e s of children before playfulness as a con struct can be further explored. Hypothesis IV Measuring Ideational Fluency Scores were obtained in a distribution which rang ed from low to high for ideational fluency. The point range, however, was so limited as to raise q u es tions as to the utility of th e methodology for furth er research. With a range of scores found in this population from l to 38, over 50% of th e population (n=44) scored in the middle range from the scor e s of 16 25. With a restricted scoring ran ge of less than 40 po ints, a majority of scores within the range of 9 points makes it difficult to utilize the instruments as designed by Lieberman (1964). (S ee T ab l e 9.) In order
PAGE 66
58 to measure the construct of ideational fluency more effec tively tasks need to be designed which better discriminate between the high and low dimensions of ideational fluency. Hypothesis V, VI and VII It was proposed that given the possibility of measur ing the three constructs adequately (ideational fluency, imaginative play predisposition, playfulness) comparisons between scores on each of the three would yield significant relationships. It was suggested that high scores on playful ness would be related to high scores on imaginative play predisposition and ideational fluency. This was suggested given the theoretical underpinnings of the constructs. Due to the limited range of scores on the ideational fluency instruments and the difficulty in establishing any subjects in the low range of playfulness, regression functions gen erated few significant relationships between imaginative play predisposition, playfulness and ideational fluency. In order to further examine the lack of significant re lationships between imaginative play and playfulness, the top twenty imaginative play scores and the lowest twenty imaginative play scores were examined in relationship to their corresponding playfulness scores (See Table 10 and 11). It was hoped that by examining this subsample of the population some further information might be gained regard ing the relationship between imaginative play and
PAGE 67
I 59 playfulness. The score utilized to determine imaginative play was the combined interview and Holtzman movement plus human response. A low imaginative play response was de fined as < 4; a high imaginative play score was defined as > 17. Graphs were plotted comparing imaginative play to a total playfulness score, a combined playfulness quantity score, a combined playfulness quality score, as well as each of the ten components of the combined scores. There were no apparent differences in the distributions. In sum mary, the only significant relationships generated by the regression equations were between imaginative play predis position and cognitive spontaneity. Discussion Extensive theoretical work and research in play behav ior in children has generated a vast and less than cohesive body of literature. Although play behavior has aroused the interest of many, there is neither a cohesive theory nor an adequate methodology to tackle this complex behavior in human children. One difficulty has been the fact that our theorizing has extended beyond adequate empirical data to support it. Another difficulty has been the diversity of approaches to the problem. Investigators have tended to follow their own isolated routes of inquiry without compar ing their work. Rather than being able to compare and con trast constructs, new ones are formed. This study attempted
PAGE 68
6 0 to provide empirical data to support two current constructs and theoretical perspectives within play behavior. Second, it attempted to compare two different approaches to play behavior in an effort to generate some unity in theoretical conceptualization. The first task, that of providing empirical support to two current theoretical constructs in the current lit erature, met with variable outcome. Jerome Singer's (1973) work on imaginative play predisposition is a relatively recent contribution to theoretical conceptualization of the role of imaginative play behavior in the development of the individual. This study, by demonstrating that imaginative play predisposition can be measured utilizing Singer's interview technique in a SES heterogeneous pop ulation of kindergarten and first graders, yields greater validity to the construct as well as reliability for the instrument with this population. The questions raised regarding the utility of the Holtzman Inkblot Test with this population are equally important. Given the high relationship between age/developmental factors and the resultant Holtzman Inkblot scores, one is inclined to doubt the utility of the instrument for this p opulation in studying imaginative play predisposition. Furthermore, if imaginative play predisposition is a quasi-personality
PAGE 69
61 trait or an aspect of personality development as Singer postulates, then one is inclined to question the Holtzman Inkblot Test as an instrument for measurement as it is so effected by age. A third question that should be raised is regarding the developmental course of the imaginative play predisposition. Is the development so rapid during these years that it is difficult to measure or is it truly a trait in the individual or only an artifact of age or development. A further question to be asked regarding methodology is that if the Holtzman Inkblot Test and the Imaginative Play Pre disposition Interview are so poorly correlated can one task be substituted for the other in inquiries with older age children where the Imaginative Play Predisposition Interview is inappropriate. The above mentioned questions underscore several needs in this area. First of all, further work needs to explore the definition of the imag inative play predisposition in order to generate a more appropriate methodology to examine it. Second, work in defining imaginative play predisposition and its methodol ogy must be examined over more populations to provide a firmer data base for the construct. Although Lieberman's {1964) results regarding playful ness were essentially replicated, i.e., teachers rated children as discriminable and in the upper range of playfulness, it
PAGE 70
62 is not possible to conclude anything from those data. With out establishing validity, other than face validity, and re liability of the construct, it is difficult to conclude what the playfulness scale actually measures. Playfulness as a construct is theoretically interesting and potentially useful. Intuitively it addresses many of the concerns raised regarding play behavior by incorporating cognitive, social and affective components. Before further empirical explora tion is possible, however, considerable basic methodolog ical research must take place in order to determine the validity and reliability of the construct. One research pos sibility would be to utilize trained observational data on children's play behavior with teachers' and/or other trained observers ratings with the playfulness rating scales. Es tablishment of adequate validity and reliability of the Playfulness scale would permit it to serve as an efficient research tool. The second problem attempted in this study, to compare two aspects of play behavior in order to provide some unity of theoretical conceptualization, yielded significant rela tionships which are intriguing. In turn these results raise further questions. Given the limited utility of the Lieber man (1964) playfulness instrument caution must be used in interpreting the results. In light of this caution, however, the relationships of significance were found between imaginative play predisposition and the cognitive
PAGE 71
63 spontaneity subset of playfulness. As the cognitive spon taneity dimension of playfulness most directly addresses imaginative play, this may in fact be the only aspect of playfulness which is related to Singer's (1973) imaginative play predisposition Of interest, however, was the fact that the slope of the regressions for the relationship be tween frequency of cognitive spontaneity and imaginative play were negative, whereas the slope of the regression representing the relationship between the quality of cog nitive spontaneity and imaginative play was positive. One might intuitively assume that the slopes would both be positive. The question on the Lieberman (1966) instrument for measuring the quality of congitive spontaneity directly ad dresses the degree of imaginatio~ demonstrated by the child in expressive and dramatic play. These significant relationships indicate an important direction for further empirical investigation. An enormously frustrating task for the individual in terested in examining play behavior is the lack of an ade quate methodology for research. Although fascinating, play behavior is complex. Theoretical conceptualization surround ing play is diverse and equally complex. The limited significant results in this study raise three basic questions. The first question is whether or not measures of the constructs have any empirical relationship to each other. The theoretical bases of the constructs
PAGE 72
64 point to a relationship which was only minimally supported by the data. The second question raised by the results of this study is whether or not the constructs a~ defined are adequate for further empirical work. The aforementioned question is a methodological concern. The evidence for re liability and validity of the instruments currently assumed to measure the constructs may be inadequate to make any clear statements regarding the existence of the phenomena. The third question raised regards the logical processes of research: the inductive process vs. the deductive process. There are two opposing schools of thou g ht concerning the most effective methods for integrating diverse data. The deductive school states that theories should be formu lated first and then tested by experim e nt. The inductive method is to experiment first and to let the theories emerge from the data. The difference betw e en the two schools is not so much the presence or absence of theory in research, but the way in which theory is developed. The deductive method provides a certain elegance in re search by which comprehensive theori e s generate the defini tion of constructs. These constructs ar e th e n o p eration a lly defined and reliable and valid instrum e nts ur e d e veloped to m e asure observable behavior. Dataare g athered in an effort to empirically support the theoretical postulates. The dif ficulty in this logical process is exemplified by the area of play. Theoretical development has far surpassed
PAGE 73
65 methodological development. In psychology where phenomena are diverse and complex this presents a serious dilemma. The current trend in psychological research is to narrow one's focus to examine a small amount of r e latively simple data. A second approach in current psychological research is to utilize the inductive method. Sidman states: intensive cultivation of an area of research by an alert ob server will inevitably bring out interrelations among the phenomena comprising the area (Sidman, 1960, p. 15). In this method a large number of experiments must be conducted with the hypothesis that order will emerge. This requires a basic faith in the orderliness of behavior. The present research most clos e ly followed the deduc tive method. Based on theoretical conceptualizations, re lationships between the constructs of imaginative play pre disposition, playfulness and ideational fluency were hypoth esized. Conclusions from the data are limited by the absence of careful methodological development. This can be seen in the lack of clear operational definitions of the constructs, and the absence of reliable and valid instru ments to me a sur e observable behavior. The course for future resea~ch can be g ui ded b y les sons from both the inductive and deductive methods of re s ea rch. Inductively more observational data need be col lected to generate th e definition of play behavior and its
PAGE 74
I 66 parameters. Deductively, the definitions of play behavior need to be tested empirically in relationship to the exten sive theoretical literature existing regarding play. Re search should establish th e reliabilit y and validity of the instruments presumed to measur e beh avio r r e lat ed to th e constructs of imaginative play and playfulness. This could be accomplished utilizing careful observation a l data on chil dren's play behavior. Investigation of childr e n's play behavior offers the researcher th e potential for greater underst and ing o f de velopment: affective, cognitive, s oc ial and p s yc hop a tho lo g ical. It is not surprising that p l ay h as captured the interest of philosophers, sociologists, anthropologists, as well as psychologists. As with so many areas in psychology, theoretical work reaches far beyond the data base. Each empirical in ve stigation provides a few small steps to close that gap. Often of greater importanc e a re th e new q u es tions which are raised. This study althou g h providing few an swers, clearly points to some important directions for fu ture research.
PAGE 75
Im aginat iv e Play Predisposition Int erview Score 0 1 1 4 2 5 3 3 4 1 67 TABLE 1 DISTRIBUTION OF I MAGINATIVE PLAY PREDISPOSITION INVENTORY ( SINGER ) FREQUENCY OVER SES LEVELS SES Level I II III IV ** (. 0 7) 3 (. 1 9 ) 2 ( .13) 3 ( .19) ( .29) 7 (. 44) 6 (. 40) 5 (. 31) (. 36 ) 4 (. 25 ) 4 ( 2 7) 5 (. 3 1) (. 21) 1 ( .06 ) 3 ( .20) 1 (. 06) (. 07) 1 (. 06 ) 0 2 ( .13) Number of sub jects. **Percentage of N for each SES level. V 3 ( .17) 7 (. 39) 4 (. 2 2) 3 ( .17) 1 (. 06)
PAGE 76
GB TABL E 2 D ISTRIBUTI ON OF COMBINED I MAGINAT I VE PLJ\Y PRED I SPOS ITI ON AND HOLTZ1'1AN INKBLOT TE S T SCORES OVER SES LE VE LS Combined SES Lev e l Score I II III I V ** 0 5 5 (. 36 ) 3 ( .1 9 ) 4 (. 2 7) 4 (. 25 ) 6 1 0 4 (. 2 9) 5 (. 3 1) 5 (. 33) 2 ( 13 ) 11 1 5 0 4 (. 25 ) 2 ( 13) 3 ( .1 9 ) 1 6 20 4 (. 29 ) 2 ( .13) 3 (. 20 ) 4 (. 25) 21 1 (. 07 ) 2 ( .13) 1 (. 07 ) 2 ( .13) Numb e r of subjects ** Percentage of N for SES l eve l. V 9 (. 50 ) 3 ( .17) 3 (. 17) 1 (. 056 ) 2 ( .11)
PAGE 77
69 TABLE 3 PLAYFU L NESS SCORE FREQUENCIES 1 5 0 6 1 0 0 11 1 5 1 1 6 20 2 2 1 25 3 26 30 9 3 1 35 6 36 4 0 25 4 1 45 26 46 50 7
PAGE 78
70 T ABLE 4 D I S T R IBUTION OF PLAYFULNESS OVER SES LEVEL P l ayfulness SES Leve 1 Score I II III IV V 1 5 0 0 0 0 0 6 10 0 0 0 0 0 11 1 5 0 1 0 1 0 1 6 20 0 0 1 0 1 21 25 1 1 0 0 1 26 30 1 2 2 1 4 3 1 35 1 0 2 2 2 36 40 4 4 7 5 4 4 1 45 4 6 2 6 6 46 so 3 2 1 1 0
PAGE 79
1 71 TABLE 5 TEACHER B PLAYFULNESS RA TIN G S >, >, >, >, >, >, >, >, _ rl _ _ rl rl rl rl rl QJ rl QJ rl ..-l QJ >, ] D' ..-l .c QJ >, QJ > QJ >, > QJ rd rd >, ~ i::: >, rl rl >, u ;.; u rl ::, ::, rl ..-l ..-l rd _ rl _ QJ QJ rl _ rl rl OI OI ;.; ;.; rl rd _ rd rl rl _ rl rl n ..Q fl i::: fl s:: ..-l 0 ..-l rl rl s:: ..-I i::: i::: i::: s:: ..-I -g 3 >, 0 >, 0 rd >, >, u u 0 rd ty\ 0 ti' o ro ..c ::, ..c ::, 0 0 ;J ::, 0 ::, 0 ::, 0 ::, 0 ::, UJ z A< UJ OI p., UJ OI rJ rJ :I: OI :I: OI U) UJ OI U) UJ OI U UJ 01 U UJ 01 1 5 4 5 5 4 5 5 5 5 5 2 4 4 5 4 4 4 3 4 4 4 3 4 4 5 4 4 4 5 5 4 4 4 3 3 3 4 4 3 3 4 5 3 5 5 3 5 5 4 3 3 2 4 4 6 5 4 5 4 5 5 4 3 3 3 7 5 4 5 5 5 4 5 3 5 4 8 4 4 5 4 4 4 3 4 3 3 9 3 3 5 4 5 4 3 3 4 4 10 4 4 5 4 3 3 4 4 3 4 11 5 4 5 5 4 3 5 4 5 5 1 2 5 5 4 4 4 3 4 3 5 5 13 4 4 4 4 5 5 4 3 4 5 14 5 5 4 5 4 4 5 5 5 5 15 4 4 5 4 4 4 4 5 4 5 16 3 4 5 5 4 3 4 4 5 5 17 5 4 4 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 18 3 2 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 4 1 9 4 3 5 4 4 4 4 5 4 4 20 4 3 4 3 3 4 4 5 4 5
PAGE 80
r7 2 TABLE 6 TEACHER A P L AYFULNESS RATIN GS >, >, >, D' >, >, >, >, . rl . . rl rl rl rl rl (!) rl (!) rl rl (!) >, rl (l) rl >, (!) >, (!) > (!) >, > (!) cu ij cu >, cu >, -;:: rl ~t rl rl >, U H u rl u ;::l ;::l rl rl rl . (!) (!) rl . rl rl QI QI H H rl cu . cu rl rl . 2 rl -n~ Ul C: UJ C rl g O rl rl @ rl C: rl C: C: C: C: rl >, 0 >, 0 cu >, >, u U O cu tJ1 0 rt! tJ1 0 cu ;::l ..c: ;::l ..c: ;::l 0 0 ;::l ;::l 0 ;::l 0 ;::l 0 ;::l 0 ;::l U) z p., U) QI p., Ul QI rJ rJ :r:: QI :r:: QI U) U) QI Ul Ul QI u Ci) QI u U) QI 2 1 4 5 4 4 5 5 5 4 5 5 22 4 4 4 4 5 5 5 4 5 5 23 2 3 3 3 3 2 3 2 4 4 24 4 2 4 4 4 4 3 3 4 4 25 3 2 3 3 3 2 3 2 4 4 26 4 2 5 5 3 2 4 3 5 5 2 7 4 4 5 5 4 4 4 4 5 5 28 4 3 3 3 3 2 3 3 3 3 29 4 3 5 5 3 3 4 4 4 4 3 0 3 3 4 4 3 3 5 5 4 4 3 1 3 2 3 3 2 2 2 2 3 3 32 4 4 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 33 2 2 4 4 5 5 4 4 4 4 34 3 4 3 3 3 2 5 5 5 5 35 4 3 4 4 3 2 4 4 4 4 36 4 4 4 4 5 4 5 5 5 5 3 7 3 4 5 5 2 2 4 3 4 4 38 3 3 5 5 4 3 4 4 4 4 39 3 2 4 4 3 2 4 4 2 2 4 0 4 3 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5
PAGE 81
73 TABLE 7 TEACHER C PLAYFULNESS RATINGS >, >, >, >, >, >, >, >, . rl . . rl rl rl rl rl QJ rl QJ rl ..-I QJ >, ..-I QJ i::: ..-I >, QJ >, Q) > QJ >, > QJ ro ro >, ro ro >, -~ ~t rl rl i::: >, u u rl u ::l ::l rl ..-I ..-I r-1 ro r-1 . rl r-1 OI OI H H rl ro . ro r-1 r-1 . r-1 rl U> C U> C ..-I 0 0 ..-I r-1 i::: i::: rl i::: ..-I i::: i::: fa i::: i::: ..-I >, 0 >, o ro >, 6 @ u o ro u o ro tJ1 0 o-, o ro ::l .c 0.. ::l .c 0.. ::l 0 3 ::l 0 0.. ::l 0 0.. ::l 0 0.. ::l 0 0.. ::l U) z p.., U) OI p.., U) OI rJ rJ ::C OI ::C OI U) U) OI U) U) OI U U) OI UU)QI 41 3 5 3 2 4 3 3 2 2 2 42 3 2 5 5 4 2 5 5 4 4 43 4 3 3 4 3 2 2 2 4 4 45 3 3 3 3 3 3 2 2 3 2 46 1 4 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 47 4 2 5 5 5 5 4 4 5 5 48 4 2 5 5 4 4 4 4 4 4 49 3 2 3 3 3 2 3 2 2 2 50 3 3 3 2 3 3 3 2 3 4 5 1 1 5 3 2 3 3 3 2 3 3 52 4 4 5 5 3 3 2 3 4 4 53 4 5 5 5 4 3 4 5 5 5 54 3 3 4 4 3 3 4 4 4 4 55 1 2 1 1 4 4 2 1 1 3 56 5 1 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 57 5 1 5 4 5 5 4 4 5 5 58 1 2 2 2 2 2 2 l 3 2 59 1 1 3 1 3 2 2 1 4 4 60 5 5 5 5 3 3 4 4 5 5
PAGE 82
. u )..-1 (l) (l) n.Q .Q 6 ;:1 ;:1 (/) z 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 D' .-I ...; (l) >, ro c: u ro -.-1 .-I . tf) C: C: >, o ro ..c 0.. ;:1 P< (I) Ol 5 5 4 5 5 5 5 5 4 4 4 4 5 4 5 5 5 5 3 4 >, .-I ...; (l) ro c: >, u rj .-I .-I tf) C: ...; >, o ro ..c 0.. ;:1 P, Ul OI 5 4 3 5 5 4 3 4 2 4 3 3 5 4 4 4 4 5 3 2 74 TABLE 8 TEACHER D PLAYFULNESS RATINGS 4 5 5 5 5 5 4 5 5 5 5 5 4 5 3 5 5 3 4 3 >, .-I ...; ro ;:1 OI 6 4 5 5 5 5 4 4 5 3 5 5 5 3 5 3 5 5 4 5 4 4 4 4 5 4 5 5 4 3 4 5 4 3 3 4 3 5 3 3 4 4 3 3 5 4 5 4 3 3 4 5 3 2 3 2 3 5 2 3 3 5 4 5 4 3 5 3 4 3 3 5 3 5 2 1 4 4 5 2 2 .G .-I (l) ..--l ,G n:l .-I .-I C: ..--l u o ro 0 0.. ;:1 (I) (I) OI 5 3 5 3 3 5 3 4 3 3 3 2 5 2 1 4 3 5 2 4 >, (l) .-I :;,(l) >, .-I C: . ro -.-1 .-I . C: C: C: o-, o ro 0 0.. ;:1 U (I) 01 3 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 5 5 5 4 4 3 3 3 4 4 .G (l) .-I :;,(l) .-I C: >, .w ro rl .-I C: C: ..--l tJ1 0 (\j 0 0.. ;:1 U (I) 01 3 5 4 3 4 5 4 4 3 3 5 4 5 4 4 3 4 2 4 4
PAGE 83
75 TABLE 9 DISTRIBUTION OF IDEATIONAL FLUENCY SCORES OVER SES LEVELS Id eationa l SES L eve l Fluency Score I II III IV V 10 0 0 l 2 1 11 1 0 0 1 0 12 0 1 0 0 0 13 0 0 0 0 1 14 0 0 0 0 0 15 1 1 0 0 1 16 0 2 1 3 2 17 0 0 1 0 0 18 1 1 0 0 0 19 2 0 1 1 3 20 2 1 2 1 0 21 1 1 0 1 1 22 1 0 3 1 0 23 0 1 1 1 1 24 1 0 1 0 1 25 1 1 1 1 1 26 0 1 0 0 0 27 1 0 l 1 1 28 2 2 0 0 1
PAGE 84
76 T ABLE 9 ( con tin u e d ) I deat i o nal SE S L eve l Flu e n cy S c o r e I II III I V V 29 0 1 0 1 3 30 0 2 2 3 2 15 2 *(.14)** 2 ( .13) 1 (. 0 7) 3 ( 1 9 ) 3 ( .17) 1 6 2 5 9 ( 64 ) 7 (. 44) 11 (. 73) 9 (. 60 ) 9 (. 5 0 ) 26 3 (. 2 1) 6 ( 3 8 ) 3 ( 20 ) 5 (. 33 ) 7 (. 39 ) *Id ea ti o nal F lu e n cy s co r es. * Pe r ce nta ge sco r es
PAGE 85
w w N N N N I-' I-' \.0 -.J w N !Subject Number \.0 0-, \.0 0-, V1 N (J) I-' I t:-< 0 I :s V1 V1 Physical Spon I H w "" "" "" w "" w w "" 1 tan e it y Quanti ty C, H I Physical Spon I I z N "" w N N "" N "" w "" "" ,i::,. ;i:, tan eity Qua lity >-3 H < ,i::,. ,i::,. V1 V1 w .t> w V1 I M w V1 V1 V1 I Joy Q uantit y I ti:, t:-< .t> V1 V1 w .t> w V1 I ;i:, V1 V1 .t> "" I Joy Qua lit y >-< ti:, I-' ti:, w V1 w w w V1 w ,i::,. ,i::,. V1 ,i::,. ,i::,. I Humor Q uantity I ~ Hi 0 i:: H I-' {/J N ,i::,. w N N V1 w w V1 .t> ,i::,. ,i::,. I Humor Q uality I ::i ti:, (1) 0 {/} Cl) {/} H Social Span I >-3 Cl) H .t> V1 ,i::,. ,i::,. w V1 w V1 w V1 V1 w l taneity Q uantity 0 0 0 z t1 (1) Cl) Social SponI {/} "" V1 "" w N ,i::,. w ,i::,. w w V1 "" I tanei ty Qua lit y M n >-3 Cognitive SponI I {/J N V1 ,i::,. V1 ,i::,. V1 w V1 .,. V1 .,. ,i::,. \ taneity Quan tity I I 8 ti:, N V1 "" V1 ,i::,. V1 ,i::,. V1 "" ,I'> ,I'> ,i::,. I Cog niti ve Spon I I tanei ty Q u a lit y C I-' ~o M Cl) w "" w w N "" w ,I'> w ,I'> "" ,i::,. I Total P la y I {/J 0 V1 \.0 (J) \.0 U1 0 U1 (J) U1 w 0 H 9 0 I-' N N N I-' N I-' N N N N N I Combined Play It H 0-, w 0 I-' 0-, w V1 ,I'> 0 U1 N 0 Q uantit y Pl C, rt H I Combined Play I t-' z I-' N I-' I-' I-' N I-' N I-' N N N <: "" N \.0 -.I w N U1 I-' (J) 0 I-' 0 Q ualit y (1) H ti:, < I-' M Imaginative Play Pl '< ti:, I-' I-' N 0 I-' I-' 0 r-' 0 0 I-' w I PredisJ?osi tion t:-< ti:, '.):s 0 w w w w Inter v i ew t1 >-< (1) ti:, I Ima g inativ e Play {/} N I-' 'O 0 ,i::,. ,I'> ,i::,. \.0 m "" 0-, V1 w 0 I-' w Predispositio n 0 H {/} en w w w w Int erview p lu s t-' ti:, w rt 0 Holtzman Movemen t t-' Cl) 0 H ::i >-3 H Imaginative Play Cl) 0 0 z Predisposition 0 t1 Cl) "" r-' N 0 I-' I-' 0 I-' w 0 I-' w I Interview p lu s (1) n [/} 0 w w w w Holtzman Movement w Human {/J LL
PAGE 86
{J\ {J\ {J\ {J\ U1 U1 .ts .ts .ts Subject Number ro a, U1 w u) w u) ro -..J U1 Ul U1 .ts I-' .t:> w ..,. ..,. Physical Spontanei ty Quantity .ts U1 w I-' U1 N N N I Physical Spon.ts tan e i ty Qua li ty Ul U1 U1 U1 w U1 w U1 U1 I Joy Quan tity U1 .ts Ul U1 I-' Ul w V1 U1 I Joy Quality "" U1 .ts .ts w .ts w .ts U1 I Humor Q uantity w U1 .ts w N w N .ts V1 I Humor Quality I '"Cl 1--' p, Social SponI~ .ts Ul w V1 N .ts w .ts .ts I tan e it y Quantity 5' (l) Social SponI {/) {/) .ts U1 w U1 I-' V1 N ..,. .ts I taneity Quality [/) t-3 () 6J 0 Cognitive Spont-< (l) M .t:> "" "" "" "" V1 N .ts Vl I tan e ity Q uantity {/) I-' 0 Cognitive Spon() ..,. U1 "" .i,. .i,. V1 N .i,. V1 1 taneity Qua lity g rt f-' ::i .:: .;:,. .i,. .ts N "" N .;:,. "" I Total Play I I (l) ..,. p. N -..J N N N U1 Ul 0 .;:,. H :3 N N N I-' N I-' N N I Combined Play I{ N N "" I-' N w N .;:,. I-' w Quantity p, rt Combined Play I r-' N N N N N I-' 1--' N I < 0 w I-' 0 u) w I-' u) I-' Quality (l) 'U I-' Imaginative Play p, '< 0 I-' w 1--' 0 "" N I-' I Predisposition 'U w w {J\ Intervi ew w w {J\ (l) r-' Imaginative Play {/) I-' I-' I 'D "" ro w N 0 0 N w Predisposition 0 {/) w w {J\ Int erview plus r-' w w {J\ ('T Holtzman Movement ,... 0 ::i Imaginative Play (/) () Predisposition 0 .ts I-' w "" 0 .ts N I-' I Int erview plus ro {/) w w {J\ Holtzman Movement, w w O"\ Human Bl
PAGE 87
TABL E 11 HI GH I MAG I NATIVE PLAY P RED ISPOSITI ON SUBJECTS: PL A Y FULNESS AND I MAG I NAT I VE PL A Y PREDISPOSITION SCORES Pla y fuln ess Scores I maginativ e Pla y Predisposition Scor es . >, >, >, >, >, i:: >, i:: >, >, I I >, ro C1l QJ C1l QJ H I rl I >, rl i:: rl i::: rl i:: r-l C: en 8 r-1 i:: en 8 QJ i:: i:: rl >, rl 0 0 rl >, >, p... 0 p... 0 ::l QJ p... 0 ::l QJ 1 &~ 0 r-l >, rl I rl 0.. i:: 0.. rl C1l C1l rl rl rl :> rl rl :> 0.. C1l >, rl s:: C1l CJ} C1l CJ} C1l rl rl QJ QJ 0.. 0 QJ 0.. 0 UJ ::l UJ ::l rl rl 0 ::l 0 ::l ::l ::l >, p... p... :> rl :> .-I ::e: :> .-1 ::e: z al a rl C1l 0..0 o.. a QJ 0 QJ a C1l .-1 en 3 rl en 3 rl en 3 r1 r-l r1 ::l ::l UJ UJ :> :> rl 'O >, 'O 0 QJ 0 C) 0 QJ n:l >, n:l >, n:l 0 a >, >, rl >, rl >, p... QJ QJ >, 0.. rl n:l 0.. rl n:l rl u u u ::l ::l rl rl . . s:: rl i:: s:: en i:: en :> E s:: UJ :> E QJ rl rl rl rl a 0 H H C1l rl C1l rl rl rl rl rl r-l rl rl rl .-1 rl rl rl H N rl .-I H N i:: n en QJ Ul QJ 0 0 rl QJ rl QJ s:: QJ s:: QJ C1l ~';J 0-, 'O QJ 0-, 'O QJ 0-, 'O QJ n:l .Q E~ E~ >, >, I:: u s:: u s:: tJ1 i:: 8' ro QJ C1l QJ rl C1l QJ rl ::l 0 0 ::l 0 C1l 0 C1l 0 C1l 0 0 ::l 0 ::l E H s:: E H i:: 0 8 H s:: 0 UJ P-. p... 1-J 1-J ::r:: ::r:: UJ UJ u u E-i ua ua H p... H H P... H ::r:: H P< H ::r:: ::r:: 1 5 4 5 5 4 5 5 5 5 5 48 24 24 28 33 29 33 2.33 5 5 3 5 5 4 3 3 2 4 4 38 21 17 11 1 8 3.0 -J 6 5 4 5 4 5 5 4 3 3 3 41 22 1 9 1 7.6 6 2 66 \.D 1 2 5 5 4 4 4 3 4 3 5 5 4 2 22 20 1 8 19 3 14 5 5 4 5 4 4 5 5 5 5 47 23 24 1 4.66 17. 66 6 15 4 4 5 4 4 4 4 5 4 5 43 2 1 22 3 1 24 3 1 6 3 4 5 5 4 3 4 4 5 5 42 21 2 1 3 7 29 4 17 5 4 4 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 48 24 24 2 1. 33 1 8 33 33 21 4 3 4 4 5 5 5 4 5 5 44 21 23 1 6 1 7 2 33 2 2 4 4 5 5 4 4 4 4 38 1 9 1 9 1 6 66 17 .66 1. 66 37 3 4 5 5 2 2 4 3 4 4 36 18 1 8 23 2 0 1 42 3 2 5 5 4 2 5 5 4 4 39 21 1 8 22 17 3
PAGE 88
-.J -.J -.J (J\ O'I lJl lJl lJl Subj ec t Number -.J (J\ w -.J 0 lJl I-' 0 lJl lJl lJl lJl lJl I-' I-' w Phy s ical Spon tan eity Quantity lJl Phy s ic a l Span.l's .l's lJl w U l N w I 1 t t an c ity Q u a i y u, lJl .l's .l's lJl I-' w w I J oy Q uantit y lJl lJl w .l's lJl I-' N N I Joy Q uality L/1 w w lJl w .l's w w I Humor Q uantity lJl w N .i::, w .i::, w w I Humor Q uality "O I-' Social SponI ~ .i::, J:> lJl w J:> N w w I tan ei t y Q uantity I-' ::i Social SpanI (l) Ul w .i::, lJl w .i:,. I-' N N I t aneity Q uality Ul >-3 C/l (l Cognitive Span0 t"-1 t"I M w w L/1 J:> lJl I-' w w I tan ei ty guanti ty (l) Ul I-' I-' Cognitive Span (l J:> w lJl lJl w w I t a n e it y Quality 0 ::i rt 1--' ::i w J:> w .i:,. N N N I T o tal P l ay I (D w I,!) N I,!) .i:,. 0 00 I,!) 0, H I Combi~ed Play ;3 N N N N N I-' I-' PJ N 0 N I-' N I,!) w lJl Q uant ity \!) 1--' ::i PJ I Combin e d Play rt N I-' N I-' N I-' I-' I-' 1--' I-' I,!) 0 00 N I-' L/1 J:> Q u a lit y (D "O I Ima gi nativ e Play I-' J:> N N N I-' w w I-' N 0 lJl I-' I-' J:> lJl w Pr ed i spos ition w w w (J\ Int e rvi e w "O w w (J\ t"I (D 0, I Imagin ative Play 1--' J:> N N I-' I-' w N I-' Ul J:> 0 J:> 00 00 I,!) -.J -.J Predis pos iti on 'O 0 w w w O'I Int er vi ew p lus Ul w w (J\ 1--' H o l tzman Move ment rt 1--' 0 ::i Ima ginat ive Pla y C/l Pr edisposition (l 0 I-' N I-' I-' w w 0 I Int erview p lus t"I (D w w w O'I Holtzman Mo v e ment Ul w w CJ'\ Human 0 8
PAGE 89
APPENDIX A IDEATIONAL FLUENCY TASKS: Introduction INTERVIEW SCHEDULE "I have some things here to do and I'd like you to help me with them. I want to find out how boys and girls do these things, and whether they have fun doing them. So, I'm going to ask you and (mention playmates where indicated) to do these with me." Product Improvement (after E. Paul Torrance) 1. "Well, what have we here. It is a toy dog. Now you tell me the most interesting, cleverest ways by which you can change this dog (or: make him different) so that you and other children would have more fun playing with it. You just tell me how and I'll write down your ideas." Record answers and comments on form. Time: l' 30" If subject obviously runs out of responses, note time and go on to Part 2. If subject can g ive no responses, skip Pa rt 2 and go on to next item. O nly where subject is ob viousl y shy, say: "All right, let's leave this for a while and maybe you'll think of something later." Return at end of battery. 81
PAGE 90
B 2 2. "What else do we have here? It's a doll. Now you go ahead and tell me the most interesting and cleverest ways by which you can change tnis doll so that it will be more fun to play with." Proceed as above. Time: l' 30" Monroe Lan g ua ge Classification Test (after C.E. Meyers et al.) "Now this is the last thing we're go in g to do." "Tell me all the animals you can think of as quickly as you can." "Tell me all the things to ea t y ou can think of." "Tell me all the toys you can think of." Record each categor y Time: 30" for each (extension to l' considered). Praise child for cooperation. "How did yo u like the thin g s we did?" Record answers.
PAGE 91
~-----------------------------------I APPENDIX B IDEATIONAL FLUENCY TASKS: GUIDE FOR SCORING This guide is divided into two parts according to the two tasks: (1) Product Improvement and (s) the Monroe Language Classification Test. While specific instructions for relevancy and scoring of responses are given separately for each task on the basis of the answers obtained in this study, the following overall guidelines for the divergent thinking factors of ideational fluency, spontaneous flexibility, and originality are ap plicable. Ideational Fluency. This is obtained by counting all of the separate relevant responses given by subjects regard less of quality. Product Improvement Relevancy of a Response 1. The response must indicate that the instructions have been understood, i.e., something needs to be done to or with the object, resulting in a change in object of situation around it. Examples: 83
PAGE 92
Relevant Make him into a fox Make him dance (If he) barked a little like alive Add another doll to make him brothers B4 Irrelevant You can play with him Share him Squeeze his hand To keep it the way it is Key back here (without follow-up) 2. The response must indicate that the child accepts the premise of change for the purpose stated, i.e., "fun to play with" and that it connects in some way with the given object. Answers indicating unrelated associational stream or pure nonsense--i.e., change to anything at all, are not acceptable. Examples: Relevant Make ears stand up Make tail wag Put bow in his head Change into rabbit (for dog) Change into doctor (for doll) Irrelevant Paper to write on or change into nothing Make into telephone Turn into table 3. A reply may be relevant for toy dog but not for toy doll and vice versa. Also, in Part 2 (doll) change to stimulus object of 1 is not acceptable. Examples: Relevant Irrelevant Make him into a lion (dog) Make him into a dolly (dog) Make him into a lion (doll) Make him into a dog (doll) 4. An answer is relevant, irrespective of underlying affect, if outcome is judged constructive. Examples: Relevant Cut off head and use the out side (clothing) to make a toy watch Poke holes in his eyes I'll twist his leg Irrelevant I'll break his head, arms, everything I'll cut off his ears Take out his eyes
PAGE 93
8 5 5. When by explicit statement or by implication, the dog or toy-man has been made alive ("adaptation" in Tor rance's categories for flexibility), the following answers are relevant: He might chase a cat Make him go to work To do things on a ship (in a number of instances sub jects called doll "a sailor") 6. Part of a reply may be accepted as relevant. Ex ample: Play with it--wrestle or (put him on his head) 6. If a child by demonstration, acting out or story telling makes changes implicit, the reply is relevant. Examples: "Woof, woof" as subject handles do g "Give him a house for children to hav e more fun play ing, and ask the postman to fix him" Scoring of Relevant Responses When scoring for ideational fluency, the unit of enumeration is the thought unit, this coinciding with the classification unit. Therefore, score as one (1) 1. When unit is logically connected and stated as such by subject. Examples: Take off shirt and change into different costume; take this ribbon off and put furry thing around him. 2. When the second part of the unit is an explanation of the first. Examples: Make him like Robin Hood--jump up and down; barked a little, like alive.
PAGE 94
8 6 3. When the second part is an alternative. Example: Change his face with a mask or skin or an animal. 4. When after an overall change has been suggested, specific changes are detailed. Example: Change into col lie--different fur, white and brown, back all brown, long mouth, sharp claws. 5. When changes are distinct but make up one context or classification unit, i.e., one idea. Example: To get a funny black and white clothes, brown hands, white nose, black eyes, and a funny green hat. score as zero (0) 1. When only agents of change are given without spec ifying any chan ge s. Examples: Make abracadabra; key back there; a fairy could change it. 2. When change back is specified. Example: Change into real pig, goose, and then change back to normal. Monroe Language Classification Test Relevancy of a Response Animals--real, imaginary, living, extinct, and generic. Examples: Relevant Cow Dinosaur Irrel eva nt Hous e s People Things to Eat--everything edible, includin g general categories and fluids as well as food specified for animals. Examples:
PAGE 95
Relevant Lamb chops Meat Grill Peanuts Milk 87 Irr e levant Ever yth ing you can buy in a store Animals Toys--a. All specific toys. Examples: Relevant Dolls Dump truck Garnes Irrelevant Doll corner Baby toys b. Objects that are prefaced by "toy" or by the sub ject saying "one could play with" or that are implicitly understood to be handled in play situation. Examples: Relevant Clock Mustache People (in the form of dolls) Telephone c. Materials for play activities. Relevant Clay Wood to make toys Examples: d. Responses with no bearing on play situation are to be considered irrelevant. Exam p les: Bees and bats Closet Toy Daphne (Name of playmate) Scoring of Re l evant Responses: Ideational Fluency Animals. No credit for duplicate responses. Count separately male and female of species, and gener ic and
PAGE 96
specific responses. tunafish. 88 Examples: Lion, Father Lion; fish, Things to Eat. a. Count separately when subject specifies different kinds of the same food. Examples: plain cheese, velveeta cheese. b. Count as sin~le re sponse when one dish or meal is implied. Examples: Spag hetti and meatballs, hamburger and french fries, supper, breakfast. Toys. a. Collective terms are counted as single responses. Example: games. b. Mere elaboration without differentiation is a single response. Examples: car, automobile car. c. Count as separate responses when sub ject specifies differences in the same toy. Examples: baby dolls, big dolls; trucks, gasoline trucks.
PAGE 97
APPENDIX C PLAYFULNESS SCALE Rating Instructions As a teacher you know that children differ in many ways--some are shy, some are friendly, some grab what they want, others ask, or wait, for it. In this study, we are interested in finding out how children differ in the way they go about their play activities--how spontaneous, how cheerful, how "full-of-the-dev il" they are, and we hope to have your cooperation in this work. Attached you will find, therefore, a rating measure made up of five scales which refer directly to a child's behavior during play. You will note that each of the five scales or questions has two parts. Part A of the question aims to get at the frequency or quantity of the trait; Part B tries to assess the quality of the trait shown. For example, "how often does the child show joy" would be the quantity of the trait, and "with what freedom of expression" would be the quality of the trait. We hope you will find it possible and worthwhile to look at the children in your group along the traits 89
PAGE 98
90 suggested in the rating scales and give us your evaluation of them. We are also interested in finding out what your impres sion is of the child's intelligence and physical attractive ness, and we would like you to give us your estimate of these as well. When you rate the children, you will, of course, want to compare them with one another as well as keep in mind a general standard for these traits in kindergarteners. It is easier and better to rate all children first on one trait (or question), and then do the same for each of the six other questions. The sheets for marking down your ratings have, therefore, been set up for the different traits. There will be twelve ratings for each child. Please put down the figure that best indicates your evaluation of the child's present thinking. Descriptive terms are also given to help you in making your rating. Any comments about the content or form of the ques tions, or about any difficulties that you may have in an swering them, will be welcomed.
PAGE 99
I. A. B. II. A 91 Playfulness Rating Scale How often does the child engage in spontaneous physical movement and activity during play? This behavior would include skipping, hopping, jumping, and other rhythmic movements of the whole body or parts of the body like arms legs or head, which could be judged as a fairly clear indication of exhuberance. Very Often Often Occasionally Rarely Very Rarely 5 4 3 2 How is his motor coordination during physical activity? Excellent 5 Very Good 4 Good Fair 3 2 1 Poor l How often does the child show joy in or during his play activ ities? This may be judged by facial expression such as smiling, by verbal expressions such as saying "I like this," or "This is fun," or by more indirect vocalizing such as singing as an accompaniment of the activity, f. i. "choo, choo train, go along ." Other behavioral indicators would be repetition of activity, or resumption of act i vity with clear evidence of enjoyment. Very Often 5 Often 4 Occasionally 3 Rarely 2 Very Rarely 1
PAGE 100
92 B. With what freedom of expression does he show his III. A. This may be judged by the intensity of loudness of a chuckle or a sing-song as well as the child's ability to repeat or resume his activity by his own choice. Very High 5 High 4 Moderate 3 Some Little 2 l How often does the child show a sense of humor during play? Very Often 5 Often 4 Occasionally Rarely 3 2 Very Rarely l B. With what degree of consistency is humor shown? IV. A. Very High 5 High 4 Moderate 3 Some Little 2 l While playing, how often do e s th e c hild show fl e xibility in his interaction with the surrounding group structure?
PAGE 101
V. 93 This may be judged b y th e child joining different groups at any one play pe riod and becoming part of them and their play activity, and by bein g able to mov e in and out of these groups by his own choice or by su gg estion from th e group members without aggressive intent on their part. Very Often 5 Often 4 Occasionally 3 Rarely 2 Very Rarely l B. With what degree of ease does the child mo v e? A. This may be judged by r eady accep t a nce of the new situ at ion, lack of distress shown over th e change, including an ability also to am use himself if left solitary after peer interaction. V ery High High Moderate Some Little 5 4 3 2 l How often does the child show spontaneity during expressive and dramatic play? Inst a nc e s of such behavior would be labeling the p la y p roducts in clay, sand or paints as they grow, and/o r changing them as a result of, f.i., a per son a l whim, an accidental sha pe or a sugg e stion from th e pee r group; similarly, in a dramatic p lay, a l abel in g of p lay roles as th e group structure develops a nd changes, f.i., extending or shrink in g a "family" as playmates come a nd go V ery Often 5 Often 4 Occasionally 3 Rare l y 2 Very Rarely l
PAGE 102
94 B. What degree of imagination does th e child show in his expressive and dramatic play? Instances of imagination would be labeling and us ing animate or inanimate objects for other than the accepted usage as well as incorporating non existent objects into the play situation. Very High 5 High 4 Moderate 3 Some 2 Very Low l VI. How bright is the child? VII. This is your estimate of the child's intelligence based on observed behavior or inferred potential. Extremely Bright 5 Moderately Bright Average Bright 4 3 2 How attractive is the child? Not too Bright l This is your evaluation of the child's physical appeal. Passabl e in Very Nicelooks and Beautiful Attractive Looking appearance 5 4 3 2 Somewhat hom e ly and unat tractive l
PAGE 103
APPENDIX D IMAGINATIVE PLAY PREDISPOSITION INTERVIEW Instructions for Interview This is a semistructured interview. Begin by making a few pleasant remarks to the child and by identifying yourself by name. Then proceed to ask each question initially as written on the sheet. appear to be understood. Repeat the question if you do not If the child replies too tersely, ask him a question such as "Tell me just how you play it?" or "How do you do it?" If the child's answer is at once clear, there is no need to pursue the question. For exam ple, if a child answers Question l by saying "blocks," we need to know whether the emphasis is on sheer construction or whether there's a "make-believe" component. A game like "marbles" or "ball" needs little further elaboration as it probably does not have a fantasy element. Write down the child's answer verbatim and also note if a further question was asked by writing (q) before the child's reply. Do not be discouraged if you get relatively brief an swers from such young children. This is to b e expected; in examining protocols we find that we are getting suffi cient material for rating purposes. 95
PAGE 104
96 Imaginative Play Interview Schedule Imaginative Play Questions 1. What is your favorite game? What do you like to play the most? (Write verbatim answer. Query if not enough information to score.) 2. What game do you like to play best when you're all alone? What do you like to do best when you're all alone? Do you ever think things up? 3. Do you ever have pictures in your head? Do you ever see make-believe things or pictures in yo ur mind and think about them? What sort of thin g s? 4. Do you have a make-believe friend? Do you have an animal or toy or make-believe person you talk to or take along places with you?
PAGE 105
APPENDIX E HOLTZMAN SCORING GUIDELINES Movement (M) The scoring of Movement is linked closely to content in most traditional systems for the Rorschach. Too fre quently such practices lead to arbitrary conventions as to whether or not movement is scored or how it is scored. The resulting score is often highly confusing from a psycho metric point of view. The essential characteristic of the movement response is the energy level or dynamic quality of it, rather than the particular content. Leaning heavily upon Zubin et al., (1953), Sells et al., (1952), and Wilson, (1952), a 5-point scale was adopted as follows: 0 no movement or static potential for movement 1 static potential for movement as indicated by such participles as sitting, looking, resting, lying 2 casual movement, such as walking, talking, climb ing, reaching 3 dynamic movement, such as lifting, dancing, running, weeping 4 violent movement, such as whirlin g e xplodin g Credit for Mov e ment is given only when th e subj e ct volun tarily ascribes movement or potential for movement to the percept. Direct inquiry to elicit movement should never be made by the examiner. The meaning of potential for movement 97
PAGE 106
98 may need further explanation. Sinc e Movement is designed to reflect the degree of movement, tension, or dynamic en ergy projected into the percept by the subject, it is im portant that concepts clearly involving tension and dynamic energy not be excluded merely because they may not be per ceived in actual motion. The potential for movement is in dicated by the stance ( 11 a lion ready to spring") or the "looking-alive" quality as described by the subject. In some cases, the state of tension may be quite high even though visible motion as such is minimal. For example, in the response, "two men struggling in a tug of war," the en ergy level is sufficiently high to merit a score of 3 on Movement, even though the men may appear relatively motion less. It is always the level of energy inv e sted in the per cept by the subject which is scored rather than the degree of motion per se. The kinds of movement list e d above to illustrate the different levels on the scale are in no way exhaustive; rather they serve to anchor the points on the scale. In scoring a given response, the particular way in which the percept is elaborated and described may occasion ally require minor shifting up or down the scale to an ad jacent category. The total score on Movement is obtain e d by sumnnng in dividual ratings across the 45 inkblots. Movement can range theoretically from Oto 180, although in practice, it rarely rises above 80.
PAGE 107
99 Content Scores: Human, A nimal, Anatomy, Sex, Abstract The scaling of content presents some difficulties because of its intrinsically qualitative nature. Inkblot responses can be classified into any number of categories according to variations in content. A single response may contain several such content categories. For research purposes a general, two-digit coding system is described elsewhere and need not be discussed further her e It is irrelevant to the variables treated quantitativ e ly in the Holtz man Inkblot Technique. Several aspects of content occur with high enough fre quency, or are of sufficient interest in their own right, to justify scaling efforts. Human and animal responses are important to record, both in order to produce such clas sical Rorschach scores as Mand FM by confi g ural scoring of Movement and content, and in order to test hypotheses of interest concerning the nature of the human and animal responses. Similarly, anatomical responses, sex references and abstractions are singled out for special attention. It is quite likely that other aspects of content may prove worthy of scaling in the future. For ea ch of thes e fiv e cont e nt scor e s, a 3-point w e i g ht ing system has been adopted, permitting a theoretical range of total score from Oto 90. These content variables differ somewhat in psychometric characteristics from most of the
PAGE 108
100 others because it is very difficult for a person to obtain for a single response a score of 1 or 2 on all five vari ables. The definition of each variable is presented be low, together with appropriate numerical weights. Human (H) Score 0. No human being or parts of a human being present in the response. Score 1. Parts of human beings, featureless wholes or distorted bodies, and mythological or cartoon characters. Score 2. Any whole human being whose parts can be differentiated. (Small parts such as a hand or foot can be missing.) The human face if elaborated. Parts of a human being, provided the subject assumes the remainder of the person to be present but hidden. Difficulty in scoring occasionally arises due to an incomplete, undifferentiated response by the subject. For example, it is impossible to tell whether the subject is referring to animal or human (or simply creature) when he gives a response such as, "an open mouth," or "a face." In most such instances, he means a human mouth or a human face; however, this assumption should not be made without support ing evidence from other more detail ed responses by the same subject. When there is genuine d o ubt, a sc ore of 1 can be g iven to both Human and Animal. Animals en gaged in activ ity which is strictly human in nature should also be scored 1 on Human and 1 or 2 on Animal. It should be emphasized
PAGE 109
101 that all five content variables have been defined indepen dently of each other so that a given response may get credit for more than one content score. The following examples il lustrate the scoring of content. Response Two creatures looking down, in mental struggle. They have a lot of tentacles. This looks like a face to me. Bunch of monkeys playing baseball. This looks like a chest cavity ... the rib cage. Big black bug crawling up a leaf. Two centaurs. An open mouth. A snake. A man walking through life with temptation next to him, leading him into sin. Face of a monster. Two dogs having intercourse. Breast with a pink nipple. X-ray of a woman's hips and pelvic region. Woman's pe lvis. Skull of a horse. A carnival ... bright colors suggest gaiety Score on Variable H A At Sx Ab l l l 0 0 l l 0 2 l 0 l l 0 0 0 l l 2 0 l l l 2 0 l 2 0 0 0 l 0 0 0 0 l 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 l l l 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 2 2 l 0 0 0 l 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 l 0 0 0 0 0 0 2
PAGE 110
102 Score on Variable Response H A A t Sx Ab Lungs of a dog. 0 0 2 0 0 Two men running. Yo u can see their 2 0 2 0 0 insides. Bacteria. 0 0 0 0 0
PAGE 111
REFERENCES Alexander, F. A contribution to a theory of play. Psycho analytic Quarterly, 1958, '!:2, 175-193. Ames, L., Metraux, R., and Walker, R. Child Rorschach re sponses. New York: Brenner/Mazel, 1974. Appleton, L.E. A comparative study of the play activities of adult savages and civilized children. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1910. Beach, F.A. Current concepts of play in animals. American Naturalist, 1945, 79, 623-541. Beller, E.K. Dependency and independence in young children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1955, 87, 25-35. Bereiter, C. Fluency abilities in preschool children. Jour~al of Genetic Psychology, 1961, ~' 47-48. Biblow, E. Imaginative play and the control of aggressive behavior. In J. Singer (Ed.), The child's world of make-believe, experimental studies of imaginative play. New York: Academic Press, 1973. Britt, S.H., and Janus, S.Q. Toward a social psychology of human play. Journal of Social Psychology, 1941, 13, 351-384. Claraparede, E. Sur la nature et la fonction de jeu. Archives de Psychologie, Geneve 1934, 34, 350-369. Corrigan, P.M. Extraversion-introversion as a dimension of personality: A reappraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 1960, 57, 329-360. Dunn, L.M. Manual for the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Minneapolis: American Guidance Service, Inc., 1959, 1960, 1965. Erikson, E. Configuration in play: choanalytic Quarterly, 1937, 6, 103 Clinical notes. 138-214.
PAGE 112
104 Erikson, E. Studies in the interpretation of play, Part I. Clinical observations of play disruption in young chil dren. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 1940, 22, 557-671 Erikson, E. Childhood and society. New York: Norton, 1950. Erikson, E. Sex differences in play configurations of pre adolescents, American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 1951, 21 (4), 667-692. Erikson, E. Growth and crises of a healthy personality. Psychological Issues, 1959, !, 50-100. Erikson, E. Toys and reason, stages in the ritualization of experience. New York: Norton, 1977. Freud, A. The ego and the mechanisms of defense. London: Hogarth Press, 1936. Freud, S. The relation of the poet to daydreaming. In Collected Papers. London: Hogarth Press, 1948. Freud, S. Be y ond the pleasure principle. In J. Strachey (Ed.), The standard edition of the complete works of Sigmund Freud. London: Hogarth Press, 1955. Freud, S. The problem of anxiety. New York: Norton, 1936. Freud, S. Creative writers and daydreaming. In J. Stracher (Ed.), The standard edition of the complete psycholog ical works of Sigmund Freud. London: Hogarth Press, 1959. Freud, S. Formulations on the two principles of mental functioning. In J. Strachey (Ed.), The standard edi tion of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud. London: Hogarth Press, 1958. Freyburg, J. Increasing the advantaged kindergarten training. In J. Singer make-believe. New York: imaginative play of urgan dis children through systematic (Ed.), The child's world of Academic Press, 1973. Gewirtz, J.L. Studies in word-flu e ncy: I. Its r e lation to vocabular y and mental ag e in young childr e n. Jour nal of Genetic Psychology, 1948, ?_3_, 165-176. Gilmore, J. Play: A special behavior. In R.N. Haber (Ed.), Current research in motivation. Holt-Rinehart, 1966.
PAGE 113
105 Gottlieb, S. Modeling effects upon fantasy. In J. Singer (Ed.), The child's world of make-believe. New York: Academic Press, 1973. Groos, K. The play of animals. New York: Appleton, 1898. Groos, K. The play of men. New York: Appleton, 1908. Guilford, J., and Christensen, P. A factor analytic study of verbal fluency. Rep. psychol. Lab No. 17. Los Angeles: University of Southern California, 1956. Guilford, J., Wilson, R., and Christensen, P. A factor analytic study of creative thinking: I. Hypotheses and description of tests. Rep. Psychol. Lab No. 4. Los Angeles: University of Southern California, 1951. Hall, G. Youth. New York: Appleton, 1906. Hartley, R., Frank, L., and Goldenson, R. Understanding children's play. New York: Columbia University Press, 1952. Hartmann, H. Ego psychology and the problem of adaptation. New York: International Universities Press, 1958. Hill, E.F. The Holtzman Inkblot technique. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1972. Holtzman, W. Holtzman Inkblot technique, administration and scoring guide. New York: The Psychological Corpora tion, 1958, 1961. Holtzman, W., Thorpe, J., Swartz, J., and Herron, E. Ink blot perception and personality. Austin: University of Texas Press, 1961. Huizinga, J. Homo ludens. Boston: Beacon Press, 1955. Hunt, J. McV. Intelligence and experience. New Ycirk: Ronald Press, 1961. Lange, K. Ergunzunstheorie. Berlin, 1901. Lazarus, M. Uber die reize des spiels. Berlin: Bun~ler, 1883. Lewin, K. A d y namic theory of personality. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1935.
PAGE 114
106 Lieberman, J. Playfulness and divergent thinking: An in vestigation of their relationship at the kindergarten level. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, 1964. Lieberman, J. Playfulness, its relationship to imagination and creativity. New York: Academic Press, 1977. Luria, A. The nature of human conflicts. New York: Liveright, 1932. Meyers, C., Orpet, R., Attwell, A., and Dingman, G. Pri mary abilities at mental age six. Child Development Monographs, 1962, 27, No. 1 (Whole No. 82). Millar, S. The psychology of play. Harmondsworth, England: Penguin, 1968. Moustakas, C. The frequency and intensity of negative at titudes expressed in play therapy: A comparison of well-adjusted and disturbed young children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1955, ~' 309-325. Patrick, G. The psycholoqy of relaxation. Boston: Hough ton-Mifflin, 1916. Piaget, J. The moral judgement of the child. New York: Harcourt, Brace and World, 1932. Piaget, J. York: Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. Norton, 1962. New Pulaski, M. Toys and imaginative play. The child's world of make-believe. ic Press, 1973. In J. Singer (Ed.), New York: AcademRorschach, H. Stratton, Psychodiagnostics. 1942, 1969. New York: Grune and Schachtel, E. Metamorphosis: On the development of affect, perception, attention, and memory. New York: Basic Books, 1959. Schiller, F. Essays, aesthetical, philosophical. London: George Bell, 1875. Sells, S.B., Frese, Jr., F.J., and Lancaster, W.H. Research on the psychiatric selection of flying personnel II. Progress on the development of SAM Group Inkblot Test. Project No. 21-37-002, no. 2. Randolph Field, Texas: USAF School of Aviation Medicine, 1952.
PAGE 115
107 Sidman, M. Tactics of scientific research, evaluating ex perimental data in psychology. New York: Basic Books Inc., 1960. Singer, J. Daydreaminq: An introduction to the experimen tal study of inner experience. New York: Random House, 1966. Singer, J. Research applications of projective methods. In A. Rabin (Ed.), Proj e ctive techniques in p e rsonality assessment. New York: Springer, 1968. Singer, J. The child's world of make-beli eve e xpe rimental studies in imaginative play. New York: Academic Press, 1973. Singer, J. The experience t yp e: Some behavioral correlates and theor e tical implications. In M. Rickers-Ovsienkina (Ed.), Rorschach psychology. New York: Wiley, 1960. Spencer, H. 1873. Principles of psychology. New York: Appleton, Terman, L.M. Ge netic studies o f genius: Vol. I. Me ntal and physical traits of a th o usand g ifted children. Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1925. Tinklepaugh, 0. Social behavior in animals. In F. Moss (Ed.), Comparative psychology, 2nd Edition. New York: Prentice Hall, 1942. Tomkins, S. Affect, imagery, consciousness (Vols. I and II). New York: Springer, 1962, 1963. Torrance. A ssessing the creative thinkin g abilities of yo ung children. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1960. Weisler, A., and McCall, R. Exploration and play. American Psychologist, 1976, 492-508. White, R. Motivation reconsidered: The concept of compe t e nce. Psychological Review, 1959, 66 297-333. White, R. Comp e t e nce and th e psychosexual st ages of devel opment. In M. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska sympos ium on moti vation. Lincoln: University of Nebrasks Press, 1960. White, R. Ego and reality in psychoanalytic theory. Psy chological Issues, 1964, l, Monograph 11.
PAGE 116
108 Wilson, G.P. Intellectual indicators in the Rorschach test. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Texas, Austin, Texas, 1952. Wundt, W. Gundriss des Psychologie. Leipzig: Kroner, 1913. Zubin, J., and Eron, L. Experimental abnormal psychology (Preliminary edition). New York: New York State Psy chiatric Institute, 1953.
PAGE 117
BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH Devora Sue Depper was born in Southern California. She received her A.B. with Honors in Psychology from Grinnell College, Grinnell, Iowa, in May, 1974. She pursued her graduate training in clinical psychology at th e University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida. She received her M.A. in 1976. She received the Florence Shafer Memorial Award for Excellence in Psychotherapeutic Counseling in 1977. Her clinical areas of interest and specialization are child de velopment and child psychotherapy. She com p leted her clin ical internship at Langley Porter Institut e University of California, San Francisco. Francisco, California. She currently resides in San 109
PAGE 118
I certify that I have read this study and that in my opinion it conforms to acceptable standards of scholarly presentation and is fully adequate, in scope and quality, as a dissertation for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Jhcqelin Goldman, Chairperson Professor of Psychology and Clinical Psychology I certify that I have read this study and that in my opinion it conforms to acceptable standards of scholarly presentation and is fully adequate, in scope and quality, as a dissertation for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Paul ~htz L Professor of Ps y chology and Clinical Psychology I certif y that I have read this study a nd that in my opinion it conforms to acceptable standards of scholarly presentation and is fully adequate, in scop e and qualit y a s a dissertation for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy. CJ ~ Q c ,-:.: J--' c, ) ... ~. ( ). --'.1 .J-< -\ '-.'._ / \ Benjainen Barger Professor of Psychology and Clinical Psycholo g y
PAGE 119
I certify that I have read this study and that in my opinion it conforms to acceptable standards of scholarly presentation and is fully adequate, in scope and quality, as a dissertation for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy. (...Marvin Shaw Professor of Psychology I certify that I have read this study and that in my opinion it conforms to acceptable standards of scholarly presentation and is fully adequate, in scop e and quality, as a dissertation for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy. ----~CC\,.; Sandra Damico Associate Professor Foundations of Education This dissertation was submitted to the Graduate Faculty of the Department of Psychology in th e Colle g e of Liberal Arts and Sciences and to the Graduate Council and was accepted as partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. June 1979 Dean, Graduate School
PAGE 120
UN I VERS I TY OF FLO RIDA \\ \ \\ \\\\\\ \\\ \\\ \\\\ \ \\\\\ \\ \1\\11 \\II I\ \11\11 Ill\ I\\ \\I \\\III\ 3 1262 08553 5127
|
|