The Role of Science in Public Understanding of Environmental Controversies

MISSING IMAGE

Material Information

Title:
The Role of Science in Public Understanding of Environmental Controversies Cognition, Media, and Resident Perception in Coastal Florida
Physical Description:
1 online resource (96 p.)
Language:
english
Creator:
Carlton, Stuart
Publisher:
University of Florida
Place of Publication:
Gainesville, Fla.
Publication Date:

Thesis/Dissertation Information

Degree:
Doctorate ( Ph.D.)
Degree Grantor:
University of Florida
Degree Disciplines:
Interdisciplinary Ecology
Committee Chair:
Jacobson, Susan K
Committee Members:
Treise, Deborah M
Pine, William
Wald, Kenneth D
Hamann, Richard

Subjects

Subjects / Keywords:
change -- climate -- content -- controversy -- newspaper -- perception -- risk -- science
Interdisciplinary Ecology -- Dissertations, Academic -- UF
Genre:
Interdisciplinary Ecology thesis, Ph.D.
bibliography   ( marcgt )
theses   ( marcgt )
government publication (state, provincial, terriorial, dependent)   ( marcgt )
born-digital   ( sobekcm )
Electronic Thesis or Dissertation

Notes

Abstract:
Controversies hinder effective coastal management, impeding communication between scientists, managers, and the public. I examined the role of science in public understanding of coastal natural resource management controversies to advance the theory and practice of environmental communications. I conducted in-depth interviews with government administrators in Crystal River, Florida, comparing their cognitive models of climate-related environmental hazards to an expert model. The models differed substantially: nonexperts were less concerned about climate change than local issues such as water quality. However, many nonexpert concerns were climate-related. Refocusing communications on climate-related hazards should promote meaningful adaptation and mitigation while lessening controversy associated with the concept of climate change. To evaluate media’s role in controversy, I analyzed the content of 441 articles in five Florida newspapers covering three coastal management controversies: a gill-net fishing ban, reef fisheries management, and climate change. I studied the use of six frames—science, scientific uncertainty, conflict, economics, human interest, and morality—and scientists’ quotations. Conflict and economic frames dominated, appearing in a significant majority or plurality of articles by controversy. Science framing appeared in significantly less than 50% of articles by controversy. Discriminant function analysis classified 63.7% of articles using two axes: scientific uncertainty and conflict. Scientists were quoted 262 times versus 532 for non-scientists. The results suggest that only a small part of media coverage is science-based and mass media may not be the best channel for science communication. To analyze coastal residents' climate-related risk perceptions, I surveyed 558 Florida undergraduates. Based on factor analyses, respondents were more concerned about risks to the physical environment (average concern 7.17/10) than risks to the economy (6.66) and biota (5.98). Regression analysis showed that environmental attitudes, gender, and politics were significant drivers of risk perception, implying that risk perception is less a function of careful thought than of individuals’ cognitive and affective characteristics. Communicators should focus on salient risks to improve climate communication. These studies demonstrate that scientific information is a small component of how the public receives, processes, and perceives coastal environmental controversies, suggesting that successful outreach must address emotional content in addition to disseminating scientific facts.
General Note:
In the series University of Florida Digital Collections.
General Note:
Includes vita.
Bibliography:
Includes bibliographical references.
Source of Description:
Description based on online resource; title from PDF title page.
Source of Description:
This bibliographic record is available under the Creative Commons CC0 public domain dedication. The University of Florida Libraries, as creator of this bibliographic record, has waived all rights to it worldwide under copyright law, including all related and neighboring rights, to the extent allowed by law.
Statement of Responsibility:
by Stuart Carlton.
Thesis:
Thesis (Ph.D.)--University of Florida, 2012.
Local:
Adviser: Jacobson, Susan K.
Electronic Access:
RESTRICTED TO UF STUDENTS, STAFF, FACULTY, AND ON-CAMPUS USE UNTIL 2013-08-31

Record Information

Source Institution:
UFRGP
Rights Management:
Applicable rights reserved.
Classification:
lcc - LD1780 2012
System ID:
UFE0044570:00001


This item is only available as the following downloads:


Full Text

PAGE 1

1 THE ROLE OF SCIENCE IN PUBLIC UNDERSTANDING OF ENVIRONMENTAL CONTROVERSIES : COGNITION, MEDIA, AND RESIDENT PERCEPTION IN COASTAL FLORIDA By J. STUART CARLTON A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSIT Y OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA 2012

PAGE 2

2 2012 J. Stuart Carlton

PAGE 3

3 To Libby, who continues to believe in me despite all available evidence

PAGE 4

4 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to thank the members of my committee for guiding me through this process and helping become an interdisciplinary researcher and thinker. I also would like to thank my committee chair, Dr. Susan Jacobson, for being the best mentor imaginable. I w ould also like to acknowledge past mentors whose guidance continues to shape me, including Dr. Oliver Houck, Dr. Hank Bart, and Dr. Cecil Jennings. I received financial assistance from Florida Sea Grant, Oregon Sea Grant, the University of Florida School f or Natural Resources and Environment, and the University of Florida Department of Wildlife Ecology and Conservation. I received technical and logistical assistance from Dr. Joan Bradsha w Cyndi Langin Thomas Ruppert, Dr. Mike Spranger, Dara Wald and a la rge contingent of undergraduates family, and friends who helped me pilot test, code, and do all sorts of other little boring things that were necessary for getting these projects done. My mother in law, Marcia Gutridge, and her husband David provided valu them Most importantly I would like to thank my mom, dad, and the rest of my family for getting me here and getting me through. Special thanks goes to Harper, who made sure that I had plenty of tim e for late night brainstorming sessions, and Libby, who helped me recover from the aforementioned late night brainstorming sessions.

PAGE 5

5 TABLE OF CONTENTS page ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ................................ ................................ ................................ .. 4 LIST OF TABLES ................................ ................................ ................................ ............ 7 LIST OF FIGURES ................................ ................................ ................................ .......... 8 ABSTRACT ................................ ................................ ................................ ..................... 9 CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION ................................ ................................ ................................ .... 11 2 COMPARING MODELS OF CLIMATE CHANGE RISKS AMONG EXPERT AND NONEXPERT STAKEHOLDERS ................................ ................................ ... 13 Climate Change Inaction: The Importance of Qualitative Research ....................... 13 Mental Models ................................ ................................ ................................ .. 16 Research Question ................................ ................................ ........................... 20 Methods ................................ ................................ ................................ .................. 20 Study Site: Crystal River, Florida ................................ ................................ ...... 20 Mental Models Interviews ................................ ................................ ................. 20 Constructing the Models ................................ ................................ ................... 22 Results ................................ ................................ ................................ .................... 23 Nonexpert Mental Models ................................ ................................ ................. 23 Comparing the Nonexpert and Expert Models ................................ .................. 24 Discussion ................................ ................................ ................................ .............. 27 Implications for Climate Cha nge Communication and Outreach ...................... 28 Implications for Mental Models Research ................................ ......................... 30 Limitations ................................ ................................ ................................ ........ 30 3 MEDIA COVERAGE OF THREE NATURAL RESOURCE CONTROVERSIES IN FLORIDA: GENERIC FRAMING AND THE ROLE OF SCIENCE ...................... 36 Media Effects and Environmental Controversies ................................ .................... 36 Media as an Information Source for Environmental Issues .............................. 37 Media as Battleground for Claims Makers ................................ ........................ 39 Research Questions and Hypothesis ................................ ............................... 40 About the Coastal Resource Management Controversies ................................ 41 M ethods ................................ ................................ ................................ .................. 42 Sample ................................ ................................ ................................ ............. 43 Data Analysis ................................ ................................ ................................ ... 46 Results ................................ ................................ ................................ .................... 47 Sample ................................ ................................ ................................ ............. 47 Framing ................................ ................................ ................................ ............ 47

PAGE 6

6 Differentiating Controversies ................................ ................................ ............ 48 Frames Associated with the Science Frame ................................ .................... 49 Use of Scientists as Sources ................................ ................................ ............ 49 Discussion ................................ ................................ ................................ .............. 50 Science and Generic Framing ................................ ................................ .......... 50 Science Frame and Scientists as Sources ................................ ....................... 53 Practical Implications ................................ ................................ ........................ 54 Future Studies ................................ ................................ ................................ .. 55 Limitations ................................ ................................ ................................ ........ 56 4 CLIMATE CHANGE AND COASTAL ENVIRONMENTAL RISK PERCEPTIONS .. 62 Climate Risk Perceptions ................................ ................................ ........................ 62 Determinants of Risk Perception ................................ ................................ ...... 63 Research Questions ................................ ................................ ......................... 68 Methods ................................ ................................ ................................ .................. 69 Risk Perceptions ................................ ................................ .............................. 69 Social Trust ................................ ................................ ................................ ...... 70 Environmental Attitudes ................................ ................................ .................... 70 Risk Salience ................................ ................................ ................................ .... 71 Demographic Variables ................................ ................................ .................... 71 Survey Administration and Data Analysis ................................ ......................... 71 Results ................................ ................................ ................................ .................... 72 Response Rate ................................ ................................ ................................ 72 Risk Perceptions ................................ ................................ .............................. 72 Social trust ................................ ................................ ................................ ........ 73 Environmental Attitudes ................................ ................................ .................... 73 Demographics ................................ ................................ ................................ .. 73 Regression Model ................................ ................................ ............................ 73 Discussion ................................ ................................ ................................ .............. 74 Risk Perceptions ................................ ................................ .............................. 75 Influences on Risk Perception ................................ ................................ .......... 76 Limitations ................................ ................................ ................................ ........ 80 5 CONCLUSION ................................ ................................ ................................ ........ 84 LIST OF REFERENCES ................................ ................................ ............................... 86 BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH ................................ ................................ ............................ 96

PAGE 7

7 LIST OF TABLES Table page 3 1 Percent present and absent frames by controversy ................................ ........... 57 3 2 ...... 58 3 3 Standardized dimension coefficients for t he discriminant function analysis ........ 59 3 4 Association between the science frames and other frames ................................ 60 3 5 Point biserial correlati on between the number of scientists quoted and frame pres ence, stratified by controversy ................................ ................................ ..... 61 4 1 Risk items divided into scales based on a principal components analysis ......... 82 4 2 Multiple regression models by risk category with risk perce ption as the dependent variable ................................ ................................ ............................. 83

PAGE 8

8 LIST OF FIGURES Figure page 2 1 Climate change expert model, developed by Flori da Sea Grant outreach personnel ................................ ................................ ................................ ............ 32 2 2 Sample hi gh level administrator mental model of climate change and coastal environmental hazards in Crystal River, Florida ................................ ................. 33 2 3 Sample department level administrator mental model of climate change and coastal environmental hazards in Crystal River, Florida ................................ ..... 34 2 4 Expert model of climate change hazards in Crystal River, Flo rida with nonexpert nodes added ................................ ................................ ...................... 35 4 1 Conceptual model of coastal environmental risk perception ............................... 81

PAGE 9

9 Abstract of Dissertation Presented to the Graduate School of the University of Florida in Partial Fulfillmen t of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy THE ROLE OF SCIENCE IN PUBLIC UNDERSTANDING OF ENVIRONMENTAL CONTROVERSIES: COGNITION, MEDIA, AND RESIDENT P ERCEPTION IN COASTAL FLORIDA By J. Stuart Carlton August 2012 Chair: Susan K. Jacobson Major: Interdisciplinary Ecology Controversies hinder effective coastal management, impeding communication between scientists, managers, and the public. I examined the role of science in public understanding of coastal natural resource management controversies to advance the theory and practice of environmental communications. I conducted in depth interviews with government administrators in Crystal River Florida, comparing their cognitive models of climate related environmental hazards to an expert model. The models differed substantially: nonexperts were less concerned about climate change than local issues such as water quality. However, many nonexpert concerns were climate related. Refocusing communications on climate related hazards should promote meaningful adaptation and mitigation while lessening controversy associated with the concept of climate change. nalyzed the content of 441 articles in five Florida newspapers covering three coastal management controversies: a gill net fishing ban, reef fisheries management, and climate change. I studied the use of six frames science, scientific uncertainty, conflict economics, human interest, and

PAGE 10

10 morality appearing in a significant majority or plurality of articles by controversy. Science framing appeared in significantly less than 50% of articles b y controversy. Discriminant function analysis classified 63.7% of articles using two axes: scientific uncertainty and conflict. Scientists were quoted 262 times versus 532 for non scientists. The results suggest that only a small part of media coverage is science based and mass media may not be the best cha nnel for science communication. To analyze coastal residents' climate related risk perceptions, I surveyed 558 Florida undergraduates. Based on factor analyses, respondents were more concerned about risks to the physical environment (average concern 7.17/10) than risks to the economy (6.66) and biota (5.98). Regression analysis showed that environmental attitudes, gender, and politics were significant drivers of risk perception, implying that risk percepti affective characteristics. Communicators should focus on salient risks to improve climate communication. These studies demonstrate that scientific information is a small component of how the public receives, processes, and perceives coastal environmental controversies, suggesting that successful outreach must address emotional content in addition to disseminating scientific facts.

PAGE 11

11 CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION As coastal populations grow and the climate changes, the pressures on coastal natural resources are likely to increase (Tobey et al. 2010). Additionally, bo th the likelihood of and consequences from environmental hazards are expected to increase in many coastal communities (IPCC 2012). However, coastal natural resource management is often hampered by political concerns, controversies over the validity of the relevant science, and poor communication. Additionally, scientists, resource managers, and other experts tend to approach coastal resource management issues differently from the lay public, relying more on analytical assessments of scenarios than more expe riential, emotion laden judgments. This can lead to poor communication between scientists, managers, and the public, which can contribute to distrust among the parties By understanding how the public views controversial coastal resource management issues, outreach and communication personnel can improve communication and management actions, framing them in a more palatable manner for the public. The three studies in this dissertation approach this problem from three different aring Models of Climate Change Risks Among Expert and differences in perception of climate change risks between expert and nonexpert stakeholders in Crystal River, Florida. S by Florida Sea Grant personnel.

PAGE 12

12 Florida: Generic F amendment, an ongoing reef fishery management controversy, and climate change. The study compares the use of six different frames in each controversy: science, scientific uncertainty, conflict, economics, human interest, and morality. Additionally, the study analyzes the use of scientists as sources of information in coverage of the controversies. The fin uses survey research to analyze coastal environmental risk perceptions among undergraduates at the University of Florida. The study also uses regression analysis to determine the extent to which cognitive and affective characteristics influence risk perception. Together, these three studies reveal critical information about how the public receives information about and perceives coastal environmental and natural resource issues. Though th e studies often raise as many questions as they answer, the work represents and important step toward improving outreach and communication in coastal communities.

PAGE 13

13 CHAPTER 2 COMPARING MODELS OF CLIMATE CHANGE RISKS AMONG EXPERT AND NONEXPERT STAKEHOLDE RS C limate Change Inaction: The Importance of Qualitative Research Climate change is a significant global risk that is predicted to be particularly devastating to coastal communities because of effects of sea level rise, coastal flooding, and increased storm a ctivity. Climate change will likely erode shorelines, raise estuarine salinity (IPCC 2007), and cause significant disruptions in marine fisheries (Cheung et al. 2009). Climate change might amplify other stresses to the coastal environment such as water pol lution, habitat loss, and overuse of natural resources (Tobey 2010). Despite the climate related risks, many coastal communities are unprepared for either adapting to or mitigating the effects of climate change. A 2008 survey of coastal states, commonweal ths, and territories in the US and Canada found that 84% of them do not have a completed sea level rise adaptation plan, and 56% have yet to begin development of such a plan (CSO 2008). A lack of appropriate physical and socioeconomic data (CSO 2008) and c ompetition over limited time and resources (Titus et al. 2009; Parkinson & McCue 2011) may hinder institutional response to climate change. Inaction on climate change is not limited to state and local governments. In a recent survey of Oregon coast profess ionals, defined as those who work private or public sector jobs near the coast, less than half of respondents indicated that they were prepared to devote time or resources to climate change (Borberg et al. 2009). The reasons for slow actions on climate cha nge are manifold, and the result of individual and societal values, perceptions, processes, and power structures (Adger et

PAGE 14

14 al. 2009). Climate change is complex, and remains a controversial topic among the public, despite a consensus among scientists (IPCC 2007; Leiserowitz et al. 2010). The public generally lacks knowledge of the existence, causes, and effects of climate change, which hinders action (Leiserowitz et al. 2010, Wolf and Moser 2011). Individuals have consistently displayed confusion about clima te change, often conflating it with other environmental problems such as ozone depletion or air pollution (Bostrom et al. 1994; Bord et al. 1998; Leiserowitz et al. 2010). Public confusion may be exacerbated by inadequate media coverage, which may be subje ct to partisan influence (Dotson et al. 2012) or overinflate the importance of the climate change controversy in an attempt to provide journalistic balance (Boykoff 2007). Incorrect or incomplete knowledge is not the only factor hindering public action on climate change. Psychological research has revealed several other challenges to action. First, climate change is a long term threat without a cause attributable to specific actions, individuals, or institutions, and people tend to ignore those sorts of ris ks in favor of those that are more salient and immediate (Stern 1992; Moser and Dilling 2004; CRED 2009). The tendency to ignore climate change might be exacerbated by the fact that the majority of US citizens tend to view climate change as a hazard that w ill affect people who are either geographically or temporally distant from the present day United States, making climate change less worthy of concern (Leiserowitz 2005). Scientists global warming largely in future oriented terms (Ungar 2000). Even if climate change is perceived as a threat, people may be slow to take adaptive or mitigating actions because of (correctly or incorrectly) perceived risks of taking such action (Gifford 20 11).

PAGE 15

15 Despite nearly 30 years of effort, climate change outreach and communication programs have not succeeded in spurring widespread public action on climate change (Wolf and Moser 2011). While some of the difficulty can be attributed to the intractability of climate change as a public issue, it has also been argued that climate change communication has been poorly designed for the task at hand. Moser and Dilling (2011) make a compelling case that climate outreach and communication has been unsuccessful bec ause of four faulty, but common, assumptions made by communicators: (1) that the public does not act due to a lack of information and understanding, (2) that the public would be motivated to act by fear, (3) that describing climate change in scientific ter ms is the most persuasive way to frame the issue, and (4) that mass communication is the best medium for communicating about climate change. The manner in which climate change communication is presented can heavily influence public reaction to the informat ion (Spence & Pidgeon 2010), and by addressing the faults behind these assumptions, coastal managers, policymakers, and climate communicators can design outreach and communication campaigns that are more likely to inspire public action. Qualitative researc h is an important, often overlooked component of designing education and outreach programs (Jacobson et al. 2006; Jacobson 2009). Qualitative research provides in depth contextual information for understanding a system and can reveal important, potentially hidden concerns that might be missed in general quantitative studies (Patton 2002). Understanding the specific context of controversial topics like climate change can help managers, communicators, and policymakers frame management strategies, communicatio ns products, and policies to reflect the reality of

PAGE 16

16 what audience perception is rather than a theoretical conception of what audience perception should be. Mental Models allo ws researchers to understand the psychological context of an issue (Morgan et al among different aspects of an issue. Mental models are simplified cognitive representations of a perceived situation (John Laird 1983). Typically, people do not think of a piece of information or a situation in isolation, but as a part of a domain of knowledge with interweaving sets of relationships among concepts. These concepts and relationshi ps are collectively called a "mental model, which represents someone's thoughts and beliefs about how about how a system works (Wickens 1984), allowing people to make predic tions about a system and take appropriate actions (Johnson Laird 1983). An individual's mental model is informed by that person's experiences, attitudes, and values (Gentner & Stevens 1983). Mental models are constructed through an iterative process in wh ich an individual receives new information and filters it through their existing models. Since existing models are a starting point for understanding novel situations, people tend to reject information that conflicts with, and accept information that agree s with, their existing mental models, though new information can cause a mental model to shift over time (Morgan et al. 2002). experiences and knowledge. This personal nature means that different people will have

PAGE 17

17 different mental models informed by different experiences and knowledge. As a result, ridden (Norman 1983). When individuals interact, they use their co ntextually relevant mental models as a basis for the interaction. In teams of individuals, mental model compatibility is a critical component of team performance (Cannon Bowers et al. 1993). Because of the individual nature of mental model construction, ex perts and nonexperts tend to have different mental models for any given phenomenon (Bostrom et l actions through their model to accurately predict the consequences of these actions (Klein 1989). The expert nonexpert gap can hinder communication, as things that the experts consider relevant and important within a system may not be relevant or importa 2004). Climate change is an area in which an expert nonexpert gap persists (Bostrom et al. 1994; Bord et al. 1998; Leiserowitz et al. 2010), which may contribute to the relati ve ineffectiveness of climate outreach. One strategy for climate outreach and communication is to find noncontroversial ways to frame climate information (CRED 2009; Hardisty et al. 2010). A frame is the 1987: 143) to a message. Focusing on mental models allows researchers to find education that are climate related but avoid the controve rsy often associated with climate change. A similar approach was used in a study of models of genetic disease

PAGE 18

18 inheritance, which found that people tended to have one of three general mental models, a fact that, once validated, could be used to design educa tional materials to aid in genetic counseling sessions (Henderson & Maguire 2000). A number of studies have explicitly compared expert models of a phenomenon to nonexpert mental models to understand the similarities and differences between the ways experts and nonexperts view a system. For example, researchers studied expert and nonexpert models of chemical hazards (Cox et al. 2003, Niewhner et al. 2004). By juxtaposing the models, the researchers were able to reveal information gaps, including areas where users were not appropriately concerned with proper chemical safety (Cox et al. 2003). This information was used in developing workplace communications, and the mental models were found to be an important part of improving such communications (Niewhner et al. 2004). This approach has been applied to risks of radon (Bostrom et al. 1992) and risks from flash floods (Wagner 2007), among others. A mental models approach was use d to compare expert and nonexpert views of climate change (Bostrom et al. 1994; Read et al. 1994). Researchers compared the mental models of adults in the Pittsburgh, PA area and found that people were concerned about climate change but were confused about many aspects of climate change, such as the difference between climate and weather. Additionally, respondents were not necessarily aware of regulatory actions being taken (Bostrom et al. 1994). A subsequent survey showed that misunderstandings such as the se were relatively

PAGE 19

19 common, and that people tended to confuse good environmental practices with specific strategies to prevent climate change (Read et al. 1994). There have been several other studies of mental models of climate change. One study using a mod ified mental models approach found that highly educated adults graduate students at MIT had mental models of climate change that implicitly violated the law of conservation of matter (Sterman and Booth Sweeney 2007; Sterman 2008). A different study focused change. The results of the study described three conceptualizations of climate related danger: human influence upon t he climate system, impacts upon natural and human communities, and threats posed to the status quo by the costs and difficulty of mitigation measures (Lowe and Lorenzoni 2007). While these prior studies provide valuable information about public perception of climate change, they have focused on students (Sterman 2007), experts (Lowe and Lorenzoni 2007) or members of the public (Bostrom et al. 1994) in inland areas. This study expands on prior work by comparing mental models of coastal environmental hazards of government administrators in a coastal Florida community to a more general, expert model of the effects of climate change developed by Florida Se a Grant extension specialists. We focus on environmental hazards generally, rather than climate change spec ifically, to attempt to find more "hooks", or common beliefs among experts and nonexperts, for use in developing climate policy, outreach and communication. In doing so, we hope to achieve the practical goal of enabling better outreach while increasing

PAGE 20

20 our understanding of how the threat of climate change relates to other environmental hazards in nonexperts' cognitive models. Research Question overlap with an expert model of climate c hange hazards? Methods Study Site: Crystal River, Florida Crystal River is a city of approximately 3300 people ( U.S. Census Bureau 2010) located on the Gulf coast in north central Florida near an eponymous river. The Crystal River King's Bay area is known for clear, spring fed waters, a sizable ecotourism industry, and has become a winter haven for the endangered West Indian manatee ( Trichechus manatus ). Crystal River is part of Citrus County, which has approximately 140,000 residents. Mental Models Interv iews Semi structured, open ended "mental models" interviews were conducted with 8 government administrators in Crystal River and Citrus County, Florida. While confidentiality agreements prevent the reporting of the specific names and job titles of the inte rview subjects, the respondents included 4 of the 12 administrators at or above the Assistant County Administrator level (up to and including County Commissioners) and 4 of the 26 administrators within relevant county departments (public works, water resou rces, etc.), for a total of 8 of the 38 (21%) of the highest administrators in the county. Previous mental models studies have shown that a small number of interviews is usually sufficient to identify most widely held beliefs about the system (Morgan et al 2002). Thus, this purposive sample should capture much of the diversity of people

PAGE 21

21 contributing to policy decisions and should be large enough to reveal many, though not all, of the relevant beliefs. An interview guide was developed based on an "expert mo del" created and provided by cooperative extension personnel and scientists at Florida Sea Grant (Figure 2 1). The authors of this study did not participate in the creation of the expert model. Since the goal of the study was to compare the models and not assess their validity, and the expert model represented the understanding of the extension specialists, no effort was made to verify the expert model for accuracy or validity. The interview guide was pilot tested with 6 members of the public unassociated w ith the study and minor revisio ns were made to question clarity. The questions in mental model interviews are intentionally vague, allowing respondents to free associate among concepts with minimal direction from the interviewer (Morgan et al. 2002). Inter views for this study, which focused on environmental risks in the city of Crystal River, began with a general question: "What comes to mind when you think of living in Crystal River, especially as it relates to the natural environment?" After exhausting th e response to this question, follow up prompts up, a slightly more directed question was asked: "Are there any hazards or dangers associated with the natural environment in Cry stal River?" This question was followed by general prompts for more information (e.g., "what else can you tell me about hazard x?", "can you explain hazard x?", "what causes hazard x?", etc.) or for additional hazards (e.g., "are there more environment rel ated hazards that come to mind?").

PAGE 22

22 Once the topics raised by the general questions were exhausted, directed questions were asked to ensure that 4 major areas from the expert model (climate change, sea level rise, storm surge, and coastal erosion) were cove red in the interview. The directed questions were followed up in a similar manner to the general questions. Interviews were recorded, transcribed, and made anonymous for coding and analysis. Constructing the Models Interview transcripts were coded for thre e types of information: environmental risks, causes of environmental risks, and effects of environmental risks. Each individual risk, cause, or effect was considered a "node" in the mental model, and nodes that were explicitly connected in the interviews w 2002). Only risks, causes, or effects that were explicitly mentioned by the respondent were included in the models. Causes could be mentioned either before or after effects, but the nodes were only connec ted if there was a direct cause effect relationship mentioned. The individual mental models were melded with the expert model to create a combined model. This model was based on the expert model and was created to assess how respondent perceptions of coast al environmental hazards matched the expert model of climate change hazards. To create the combined model, each node and link on the expert model was marked with the number of interviewees (i.e., up to 8) who had the node in their mental model of coastal e nvironmental hazards. Additionally, any nodes on the expert model was added as a node to the combined diagram. When possible, similar concepts were combined into a single node to aid in interpretation. The combined model gives a visual sense of the overlap and discrepancy between the

PAGE 23

23 expert model and nonexpert mental models. All model diagrams were created using OmniGraffle Pro 5 (The Omni Group, Seattle, Washington). Resul ts The results are presented in an ordered adapted from suggestions made by Morgan et al. (2002), with two sample mental models presented before the combined model with expert and nonexpert nodes. Nonexpert Mental Models The first sample mental model came from a high level county administrator and consisted of 29 nodes with 35 links (Figure 2 2 fit within an existing node) onto the expert model: climate change, sea level rise, coastal erosion, drought, aquifer drawdow n, severe weather extremes, hurricanes and non reduced ecotourism, and economic harm. Fifteen of the nodes did not map to the expert model: God, natural cycles, coal power, and greenhouse gases as causes of climate nclude any causes of climate change); melting ice manatee population increases, manatee waste, development, septic tanks, fertilizer use, and aquatic vegetation chan ges as a cause of water quality reduction; and property value declines and increasing insurance rates as a cause of economic harm. The second sample mental model came from a departmental administrator who works in a natural resource related department (Fig ure 2 3). The model comprised 37 nodes with 49 links. Seventeen nodes mapped onto the expert model: climate change,

PAGE 24

24 sea level rise, coastal erosion, severe weather extremes, drought, hurricanes and non and infrastructure damage, human life endangerment, loss of coastline, saltwater reduced ecotourism and aquatic recreation, and economic harm. Twenty nodes did not map to the expert model: greenhouse gases as a cause of climate change; terrestrial vegetation changes and natural limestone loss as a cause of coast al erosion; lack of Bay water quality reduction; manatee population increases, aquatic vegetation changes, boat traffic, septic tanks, increases in aquatic nitrate levels, fertilizer use, nutrification, algae blooms, and shoreline hardening as causes of water quality reduction; increases in aquatic parasites, human health problems, invasive aquatic plants, and loss of native plants as an effect of water quality reduction, a nd property value declines and increasing insurance rates as causes of economic harm. Comparing t he Nonexpert and Expert Models Including the four discussion guide topics, 24 nodes on the expert model also appeared in one or more of the nonexpert mental mo dels (Figure 2 4). These nodes were mentioned by an average of 4.83(SD=2.52) respondents. The nodes included increasing climatic variability, increased water temperature/CO2 levels, increased rates of sea level rise, longer dry periods, increased storm int ensity (note: any storm or hurricane nodes were mapped to this node), increased coastal flooding, increased coastal erosion, saltwater intrusion, decreased surface water availability, increased storm surge (all storm surge nodes mapped to this node), loss of public & private

PAGE 25

25 beaches, change in invertebrate distributions, need to develop potable water, increased damage to public infrastructure, loss of private property, loss of life, loss of tourism dollars, damage to local fishing economy, public armoring, loss or residential property, loss of business property, private armoring, increased costs to the public, and decreased quality of life. The most frequent of the nodes common to both models are described below. Increased storm intensity and storm surge: Hu rricanes and other storms were a primary concern of respondents, both in terms of wind damage and storm surge, as described in this quote from a high ranking elected official: The Sheriff's Office, who handles emergency management for Citrus County, put ou t their annual hurricane guide a couple years ago, and the cover of that was a picture of this building with a storm surge about five feet above it. (With) that really major storm surge, like a Category 4, Category 5 storm, this whole town goes under water Even though we're seven miles removed from The Gulf, it's all flat. There's nothing to really stop high water, other than the seven miles. We would have very significant difficulty if we got that kind of a storm surge. Loss of private property: Respondents were concerned about property and infrastructure damage to homes and businesses, particularly from hurricanes and other storm events: A storm surge [from a] Category 2 (hurricane) is gonna wipe out a huge percentage of th e homes...the [new homes] that are up on pilings ten feet above sea level, probably will survive, but we'll have huge infrastructure disruption. The coastal roads will be wiped out. Loss of tourism dollars: Respondents were concerned about water quality an d other environmental issues causing a reduction in ecotourism, such as in this quote: [A]s the water quality declines, we may lose some of that tourist base, and there's nothing to step up and replace that. And that's a clean industry. And since we have r etirees, we don't have a good diverse economic base at this time, [loss of ecotourism] is a real concern to everybody. We'd like to keep that, because that's a clean industry.

PAGE 26

26 Increased costs to the public: Any mention of economic harm to individuals or th e local economy was coded to this node. Respondents were concerned about a general degradation to the economy resulting from reduced ecotourism, severe hurricane or storm damage, and property value declines, as illustrated in this quote from a department l evel administrator: At some point in time, either through man's actions such as regulation and controls or through environmental degradation, the ecotourism is going to start to drop off A nd that's a major component in this economy. Seven nodes were only raised by nonexperts but still fit within the expert model (SD=1.68) respondents each. T he nodes included: water quality loss in Crystal vegetation changes, the spread of invasive aquatic plants, increased insurance rates, and decreased property values. The most commonly mentioned of these are described below. Reduced water quality in Crystal River/King's Bay: Respondents expressed concern over water quality in Crystal River, King's Bay, and nearby springs, either due to a loss of water clarity or other probl ems: For anybody who has been here for a fairly reasonable amount of time, it's obviously degraded significantly from what they were used to, or what they poor. The bottom has gotten a lot of sedimentation. Property value declines: Many respondents expressed concern over current and future property value declines, especially as resulting from the effects of water quality reduction, such as in this quote from a department level adminis trator :

PAGE 27

27 [W]hen that river was gin clear, and the water was crystal blue [waterfront properties] would be a jewel. And now, when [the Crystal River] is just green, pea green, it's just a regular river, and [the property is] not as valuable. Aquatic vegetati on changes: Aquatic vegetation (including algae and algal blooms) was a concern for many respondents, especially as related to water quality. Often, respondents were concerned about the relationship between aquatic vegetation and manatees, as stated by a c ounty level administrator: I know that when we think about manatees, they're a wonderful thing to look at, and they're a big attraction, but what are they doing about the natural grasses that they're eating, that clean and filter the water? You can't grow it back fast enough. They did a replanting in (a nearby spring), and after eight or nine months the (manatees) knocked it down and ate it, so there wasn't enough time to get a natural replanting growth back in there. Terrestrial vegetation changes: Respon dents were concerned with changes in terrestrial vegetation from sea level rise and climate change: We are seeing vegetation that has a low salinity tolerance dying off, and many of those are canopy type trees. And so, we're seeing that vegetation shift. W hen those trees die off, they in turn present opportunities for invasive species like Brazilian Pepper to come in. Increased insurance rates: Respondents were concerned about increased property insurance rates, particularly in response to flooding or hurri cane risks: can tell you this, if you live w est of (US Route) 19, your chances of getting a policy are slim to none and you're gonna pay through the nose. Discussion The interwoven sets of causes and effects that make up a mental model can contain relationships that are explicit and direct or implic it and indirect. By comparing nonexpert mental models of environmental hazards to an expert model of climate

PAGE 28

28 change, this study showed how considering the relationships between the models allows communicators and policymakers to find areas of commonality i n otherwise controversial topics. The results suggest that a path to effective climate outreach and communication may lie in focusing on non controversial areas that may be only indirectly related to climate. When comparing the models, the combined nonexpe rt models did not map perfectly onto the expert model, which is typical for mental models studies ( e.g. Bostrom et al. 1994; Cox et al. 2003; Niewhner et al. 2004). Most of the climate change impacts that were common between the models were related to h urricanes, group of four of five were absent in the expert model. Many of these ecological concerns revolved especially manatee populations. The higher frequency of the unique nonexpert nodes mentions per node) supports prior research showing that people in the US tend to view climate change as less salient than other environmental concerns (Leiserowitz 2005). Im plications for C limate C hange C ommunication and O utreach mental model, it does give a general sense of the understandings of a diversity of people who contribute to policy decisio ns in Crystal River. The combined model illustrates the compatible parts of the expert and nonexpert models, which prior research has shown to be a critical component of groups working well together

PAGE 29

29 (Cannon Bowers et al. 1993). Focusing on these compatible parts reveals a number of potential points of entry for successful climate change outreach and communication despite the substantial differences between the expert and nonexpert models. Prior research has indicated that people tend to ignore threats that are distant, general, or not caused by specific, identifiable actions (Stern 1992; Moser and Dilling 2004; CRED 2009). Climate outreach and communication should focus instead on some of the specific, local concerns that people are psychologically more incl ined to act upon. The nonexpert model reveals that many of these concerns, such as water quality and hurricane preparedness, are simultaneously related to climate change allowing managers and outreach personnel to tie climate change outreach to other, mor e salient topics. Involving stakeholders to create collaborative, cultural models of risk as opposed to a one way, technical model, can improve risk communication (Cox 2006). Focusing on locally salient issues found by comparing mental models to expert mo dels can be an important form of collaborative risk definition and communication The nonexpert models have other implications for climate change education and outreach. The nonexpert models revealed information that would not necessarily be apparent to di stant researchers or communicators, or be a part of the more general expert model, which was designed to be applicable throughout Florida. While some of the nodes may seem obvious after the fact, there is a danger in making assumptions. For example, water quality, which was one of the most common and important nodes raised by the nonexperts, was a minor focus in the expert model, yet at a local level is a consistent source of concern and action. Additionally, hazards that might have initially seemed importa nt to external sources were not necessarily so in the local mental

PAGE 30

30 models. For example, the risks associated with fish population declines did not seem salient or important to respondents compared to other hazards. This was a somewhat surprising result, as Crystal River is a coastal community with an active fishing industry. Because it is impractical for communicators and policymakers to have detailed knowledge of every community, it is important to incorporate local qualitative research when working with c ommunities. Implications for Mental Models Research Much of the research using mental models has focused on aiding communication related to a single risk phenomenon, such as radon exposure (Bostrom et al. 1992), chemical hazards (Cox et al. 2003, Niewhner et al. 2004), or flash floods (Wagner 2007). Even the early work on climate change mental models (Bostrom et al. 1994; Read et al. 1994) had a fairly narrow focus on specific climate change phenomena. This study took a different approach, expanding beyond the hazards of climate these hazards to an expert model. By examining the broader context of the relationship between the risks of climate change and other risks, new connect ions were revealed and the number of potentially relevant nodes was expanded. Studying mental models enables more effective outreach and communication by helping communicators identify, as Kaplan (2000) termed it, multiply desirable solutions. A mental mod els approach should be useful in other situations involving controversial phenomena when, as with climate change, there is significant disagreement about even the most basic concepts. Limitations This study has several limitations. As qualitative research with a small number of individuals in one coastal community in Florida, the specific results cannot be

PAGE 31

31 generalized. Additionally, the difference in the process of creating the expert model and creating the nonexpert models may have contributed to some of t he discrepancies between the models. The nonexpert models were created from a single interview with each respondent, and likely do not contain all of the information known by the subject about climate change and other coastal environmental hazards. Additio nally, some detail in the nonexpert models may have been lost as specific concepts were combined into general nodes. However, I believe that the models created in these interviews are a reasonable representation of the aspects of coastal environmental haza rds that were most salient to the respondents. While the similarities and differences between the expert and nonexpert models might not be canonical, they are instructive, and point to the critical importance of on the ground research when designing effect ive outreach and policy.

PAGE 32

32 F igure 2 1. Climate change expert model, developed by Florida Sea Grant outreach personnel. Arrows indicate a causal or influential relationship.

PAGE 33

33 F igure 2 2. Sample high level administrator mental model of climate change an d coastal environmental hazards in Crystal River, Florida. NOTE: Shaded nodes were raised by the interviewer and not otherwise by the respondent

PAGE 34

34 Figure 2 3. Sample department level administrator mental model of climate change and coastal environmenta l hazards in Crystal River, Florida. NOTE: Shaded nodes were raised by the interviewer and not otherwise by the respondent

PAGE 35

35 Figure 2 4. Expert model of climate change hazards in Crystal River, Florida with nonexpert nodes added.

PAGE 36

36 CHAPTER 3 MEDIA COVERAGE OF THREE NATURAL RES OURCE CONTROVERSIES IN FLORIDA: GENERIC FRA MING AND THE ROLE OF SCIENCE Media Effects and Environmental Controversies Environmental and natural resources controversies are often complex, technical, and difficult for the public to fully understand (Weible 2007). Given this inherent any new issue (Fiske and Taylor 1991), intermediaries such as media are critical sources of public information related to these types of controversies (Lee 2004). This study analyzes media coverage of three natural resource management related controversies in Florida: a 1994 net fishing ban, an ongoing controversy over reef fish management, and climate change. Mass media serv e as more than a simple information provider. Decades of research have shown that the amount and type of media coverage an issue receives can influence public perception of the issue (Bryant and Oliver 2009). Three separate, but related, media effects that have been studied are the agenda setting, priming, and framing effects (Scheufele and Tewksbury 2007). Agenda setting occurs when the amount of media coverage an issue receives influences the importance of that issue in content suggests to news audiences that they ought to use specific issues as and Tewksbury 2007:11). Framing, the framework of this s tudy, is an aspect of media coverage that can for the news content that supplies a context and suggests what the issue is through the

PAGE 37

37 use of selection, emphasis, excl 101). such as conflict, timeliness, or novelty, socially salient themes, the presence of recognizable personalities (Price and Tewksbury 1997), and organizational pressures (Gans 1979). The frames chosen can serve one of several implicit functions: defining causes, diagnosing causes, making moral judgments, or suggesting remedies (Entman 1993). Framing is related to agenda settin g in that frames can either explicitly or implicitly suggest causality or responsibility for the events described in the article (Maher 2001). The implied causality can alter the manner in which the audience perceives an issue (McCombs and Estrada 1997). T defines and judges problems and actors while changing audience perception of potential solutions to the problems (Entman 1993). While framing theory is still developing (for a review, see Tewksbury and S cheufele 2009) there have been numerous studies analyzing how different issues have been framed. Frame analysis is a way to study the potential influence of media coverage on an audience. Researchers analyzing the use of frames can reveal patterns of news coverage, compare the way issues are covered, and, in cases of controversial issues, determine if any particular stakeholder group or viewpoint dominates media coverage of the controversy (Miller and Riechert 2001; Siemer et al. 2007). Media as a n Informa tion Source f or Environmental Issues Environmental and natural resources issues tend to be unobtrusive (Eyal et al. for which they rely on media as the primary, and s

PAGE 38

38 (Ader 1995: 300). The public often relies on mass media for information on unobtrusive issues, which may strengthen media effects associated with them (Zucker 1978; Atwater et al. 1985; Lee 2004). Understanding how uno btrusive issues are framed and the influence of that framing on public perception, therefore, may be particularly important. For example, media coverage has been an important source of information about climate change (Wilson 1995; Nisbet 2009). However, t he role of media coverage of climate change has been controversial, as studies have shown that coverage may be influenced by politics (Carvalho 2007; Dirikx & Gelders 2010; Dotson et al. 2012). Additionally, journalistic norms such as balance (i.e., presen ting both sides of an issue) and news values such as relevance, controversy, or proximity may cause media to give undue weight to climate skepticism even as the scientific evidence for anthropomorphic climate change grows (Boykoff 2007). The potential effe cts of this coverage are great, because media act as critical validators of science, especially in cases of controversy (Gamson 1999). Frame analysis of mass media coverage of the environment, fish and wildlife, and natural resources issues is a burgeoning field. In wildlife related issues, many recent human wildlife conflict, often focusing on a single species or several closely related species. These studies have found that media coverage can contribute to public knowledge about and perceptions of a number of species, including cougars (Wolch et al. 1997; Jacobson et al. 2012), black bears (Corbett 1992; Gore et al. 2005; Siemer et al. 2007), and cormorants (Muter et al 2009), among others.

PAGE 39

39 These prior studies of both environmental and wildlife issues typically have focused on risks or frames that are specific to the individual being studied. Such a focus is informative in aggregate. However, there are also cross issue, inherent in nearly every news story (De Vreese 2005). Generic frames have been variously defined depending on the focus of a given study. For example, strategic framing, which is news framing that focuses the mechanics of winning and losi ng political battles, has been found to be a consistent feature of political and policymaking all news coverage, it is generic to these broad issues. Other generic frames are gener ic to most, if not all, news coverage. One proposed list of generic frames includes conflict, economic consequences, human interest, morality, and attribution of responsibility (Semetko and Valkenburg 2000; De Vreese 2005). Studying the use of generic fram es across issues may enable researchers to compare media coverage more easily, potentially enabling theory development and complementing studies of issue specific framing. Additionally, understanding how generic frames are related to the use of a science f rame could inform the issue and background d research that is an essential part of developing an education or communication campaign (Jacobson 2009). Media as Battleground f or Claims Makers Given the power of media and the fact that journalists often must rely on outside sources for information, claims and Kitsuse 1977: 75, quoted in Trumbo 1996), are often an important asp ect of news coverage of an issue. Claims making in media stories often takes the form of direct quotations from sources (Trumbo 1996); the sources who are selected are considered

PAGE 40

40 successful claims makers. Successful claims making can promote a certain prob lem definition for a given issue, potentially altering the operating environment for risk communication campaigns (Gore and Knuth 2009). Government sources, including scientists, often play a critical role in media coverage of wildlife. Corbett (1992) stud ied wildlife stories in Minnesota and found that a significant majority of the sources cited were bureaucratic (government employees, rather than specific controversies, it included stories about wildlife related controversies. Jacobson et al. (2012) found a similar reliance on government sources, including scientists, in media coverage of Florida panther. The use of sources may change over time or with the tenor of the co verage. Trumbo (1996) studied the use of sources in national newspaper coverage of climate change and found that the relative number of scientists quoted decreased as the issue became more politicized. Wolch et al. (1997) implied, though did not measure, a similar phenomenon in coverage of mountain lions in the Los Angeles Times finding that as coverage of mountain lions grew increasingly negative, stakeholders on both sides of the cougar management controversy used the opportunity to promote their viewpoi nt through claims making in news stories. Though Wolch et al. did not report specific numbers, their findings indicate a potential mechanism by which scientists decrease as a source of quotes: as more groups compete for attention, journalists may try to in corporate more points of view, subsequently reducing the attention paid to scientists. Research Questions and Hypothesis This study analyzes the use of generic frames and science frames in three coastal resource management controversies to see if there ar e common themes among them.

PAGE 41

41 Additionally, this study is interested in the use of scientists as sources in media coverage of the controversies, as the prevalence of scientists as claims makers might influence future decisions about science and risk communic ation in controversial systems. Based on the research reviewed above, this study asks the following research question and tests the following hypothesis: RQ1: How were the three controversies framed in terms of science frames and generic frames? RQ2: To wh at extent are scientists used as sources and does the prevalence of scientist sources vary by frame? H1: Scientists will be a minority of total sources quoted in each of the three controversies. About the Coastal Resource Management Controversies The three controversies were chosen for their relatively extensive media coverage, their statewide relevance, and their representativeness of the different types of coastal natural resource controversies. There is a consistent tension between recreational and comme rcial fishers in itself in several large controversies over the years. The first controversy of interest in this study is the 1994 voter referendum to amend the Florida cons titution to ban the use of gill nets and other entanglement nets in Florida waters. While putatively about saving mullet populations ( Mugil spp. ) from overfishing and reducing bycatch of other fishes, including popular sportfish such as spotted sea trout ( Cynoscian nebulosus ), there was little scientific evidence that an action as drastic as banning nets was necessary to accomplish these goals. However, net ban proponents were able to rally citizens under from a resource allocation problem

PAGE 42

42 into an environmental problem. As a result, the net ban passed with 74% of the vote. In the time since the net ban, mullet populations have rebounded slightly while many former mullet fishers were put out of work (Adams e t al. 2000). Reef fishery management, particularly in the grouper and snapper fisheries, is a current management controversy in Florida. Red grouper and red snapper are heavily fished by both recreational and commercial fishers, and the populations may be severely overfished (Coleman et al. 2004). However, the population dynamics of grouper and snapper are extraordinarily complex, and often under fire from both recreational and commercial fishers. The technical difficulty of determining population size, a m anagement scheme with different rules in federal and state waters, and attitude and value differences between commercial and recreational fishers have led to a management controversy, threatening both the fishery and the legitimacy of the management regime (Smith 2006 ; Cowan et al. 2010 ). The third coastal controversy of interest is climate change. Climate change is a significant global risk that may be particularly devastating to coastal areas due to the effects of sea level rise and increased storm activi ty. Climate change is likely to cause increased flooding, erosion, and changes in salinity levels (IPCC 2007), potentially amplifying other coastal stresses such was water pollution and overuse of resources, as well (Tobey et al. 2010). Methods Content ana This study used content analysis to characterize the frames used in newspaper

PAGE 43

43 coverage of the net ban amendment, the reef fish management controversy, and the controversy surrounding climate change. Sample A different sample of at least two years was selected for each controversy. The samples were chosen to balance efficiency with thoroughness while captu ring a high leverage, salient time for each controversy. For the net ban, the sample consisted of a census of net ban related news items and features published between March 1, 1992 (roughly when the public movement to ban the nets began) and November 8, 1 994 (the date of the vote) in four newspapers: the Miami Herald (average 2012 Monday Friday circulation: 160,988; this and other circulation figures from the Audit Bureau of Circulations 2012), Orlando Sentinel (173,576), St. Petersburg Times (since rename d the Tampa Bay Times 299,497), and Tampa Tribune (144,510). The sample was found electronic databases. Articles that were not related to the net ban controversy were eliminated from the sample. The reef fish management sample consisted of a census of news items and features published between June 1, 2004 and December 31, 2010 in five newspapers: the Florida Times Union (average 2012 daily circulation: 98,580), the Miam i Herald the Orlando Sentinel the Sarasota Herald Tribune (79,845), and the St. Petersburg Times The beginning date for this sample was selected to coincide with the introduction of Reef Fish Amendment 22, establishing a rebuilding plan for red snapper. The end date for this sample was chosen because it approximated the present date when the sample was chosen and coincided with the passage of new, more restrictive snapper regulations in mid

PAGE 44

44 management of the reef fish populations were eliminated from the sample. is a sample in which all of the relevant articles are taken from one randomly selected Sunday, one randomly selected Monday, one randomly selected Tuesday, etc., per year. Prior research suggested that two constructed weeks per year can be sufficient to s overall content (Lacey et al., 2001). However, since this project was concerned with a specific issue, a larger sample of 10 constructed weeks per year was selected for each year from 2006 to 2008 from the same five newspapers as the reef management samp le. Ten weeks per year is approximately 20% of the issues in any given year. 2006 was selected because it was the year in which An Inconvenient Truth was released in the United States, spurring public interest in climate change (Boykoff 2007), and 2008 was selected as end date because it coincided with the end of a US Presidential election, which may have precipitated additional climate ses as described above. Letters, editorials, and opinion pieces were not sampled for any of the three controversies because the goal was to characterize the frames that a reader might find in putatively neutral coverage. Each individual article was coded a s a distinct coding unit. Articles from all three controversies were coded for the presence or absence of two issue specific frames, science and scientific uncertainty, and four generic frames, as identified and operationalized by S emet ko and Valkenburg (2 000). The science and scientific

PAGE 45

45 uncertainty frames were operationalized based on a pilot study of related articles from different newspapers. The frames were operationalized as follows: Science : A science frame was present if the article mentioned science or scientific studies as a way of explaining a phenomenon. Scientific uncerta inty: A scientific uncertainty frame was present if the article described the science related to an issue as uncertain, wrong, incomplete, or invalid. Conflict: A conflict frame was present if the article reflected disagreement between parties, individuals, or groups, or if one individual or group reproached another, or if opposing sides of a problem or issue were referred to. Economic consequences: An economic consequences frame was present if the article mentioned costs associated with a course of action, the (positive or negative) economic consequences of pursuing a course of action, or financial gains or losses. Human interest: A human interest frame was present if the story pr ovided a human example or placed a human face on an issue, employed adjectives or personal vignettes that generate feelings of outrage, caring, empathy, or compassion, or focused on the private or personal lives of the actors. Morality: A morality frame wa s present if the story contained an overt moral message, referred specifically to morality, God, or other religious tenets, or offered specific social prescriptions about how to behave. A fifth frame, attribution of responsibility, was dropped from the ana lysis for insufficient inter coder reliability.

PAGE 46

46 Additionally, the number of sources quoted was recorded for each article. Sources were classified as either lay, science, interest group, or government. Government workers identified as scientists were classi fied as scientists. The articles were coded by the author in two readings. During the first reading, the descriptive information about the article (author, article type, w ord count, and newspaper). The second reading was used to identify the frames used. A subsample of 15% of the articles (rounded to the nearest article) was coded by a second coder for the purpose of establishing intercoder reliability. Following Krippendor ff (2004) and Siemer variables with a Pi under 0.7 were dropped from the analysis. Statistical analyses were performed with SPSS version 20. Results were considered sta tistically significant at a=0.05. Intercoder reliability was good to excellent Data Analysis Each frame was scored as present or absent in each article. A binomial test was used to determine if frames were more or less likely to be present than expected by random chance (i.e., 50% chance of being present, 50% chance of being absent) for with binary outcomes (Con over 1998 ), was used to determine which frames were most A discriminant function analysis was performed to determine if the presence of the frames could be used to predict which controversy a story covered. The discriminant function analysis was validated using a leave one out cross validation and assessed

PAGE 47

47 using the proportional chance criterion, which assumes a random classification of nchez 1974). All articles were included in the analysis. The association between the science frame and other frames was assessed using the phi coefficient, which measures the degree of association between two binary variables and is interpreted similarly t o the Pearson correlation coefficient. Associations between the number of scientists and the frames, stratified by issue, were measured using a point biserial correlations. Results Sample The final sample consisted of 441 articles: 89 about the net ban, 10 8 about the reef fishery, and 244 about climate change. Framing The presence and absence data for the frames varied by controversy (Table 3 1). The conflict frame was present in significantly more than 50% of articles about the net ban and reef fishery; t he economic frame was present in significantly more than 50% of articles about the reef fishery and significantly less than 50% of articles about climate change. The human interest frame was present significantly less than 50% of the time in all three cont roversies; the science frame was present significantly less than 50% of the time in net ban and climate change articles. Morality was present in significantly fewer than 50% of articles about all three controversies; scientific uncertainty was present in s ignificantly fewer than 50% of articles about the net ban and climate change. The dominant frames differed somewhat by controversy (Table 3 2). For all three controversies, the conflict frame was the most dominant frame. The conflict frame was

PAGE 48

48 in its own Q group in the net ban and fishery articles, and grouped with (i.e., was similarly dominant as) the economic frame, human interest frame, and science frame in climate change coverage For the net ban, the least dominant frames were science, morality, and un certainty. For the reef fishery, the morality frame was least dominant. For climate change, the morality and uncertainty frames were least dominant. Differentiating Controversies Two discriminant functions were calculated, both of which were statistically significant (Table 3 3). The first discriminant dimension was strongly positively weighted by the uncertainty frame, slightly positively weighted by the science, conflict, economic, and morality frames, and negatively weighted by the human interest frame. The canonical correlation for dimension one was 0.44 (chi sq=168.53, 12 df, p<0.001). The second discriminant dimension was strongly positively weighted by the conflict frame, positively weighted by the human interest frame, and negatively weighted by the uncertainty, morality, science, and economic frames. The canonical correlation for dimension two was 0.39 (chi sq=73.05, 5 df, p<0.001). These results indicate that the first dimension reflected a scientific uncertainty/lack of uncertainty axis and the sec ond dimension represented a conflict/lack of conflict. The group centroids for functions (1,2) were ( 0.14, 0.84) for the net ban, (0.86, 0.11) for the reef fishery, and ( 0.33, 0.26) for climate change. The discriminant functions correctly classified 63 .7% of the original cases. A leave one out cross validation correctly classified 61.0% of the cases, indicating a good fit. Classifying the articles based on the discriminant functions results in a 56.5% improvement over randomly assigning articles to grou ps using the proportional chance criterion, which would expect to classify 40.7% of articles correctly.

PAGE 49

49 Frames Associated with the Science Frame The analysis revealed few significant associations between the science frame and other frames (Table 3 4). Ther e were no significant associations between the science frame and any other frames in net ban coverage. In reef fishery coverage, there was a weak positive association between the science frame and the uncertainty, conflict, and economic frames. In coverage of climate change, there was a weak negative association between the science frame and the economic, conflict, and morality frames. Use o f Scientists as Sources Scientists made up a minority of sources quoted. A total of 31 science sources were quoted in articles related to the net ban, averaging 0.35 sources per article. This compares to 163 non scientists quoted, an average of 1.83 per article. The reef fishery had similar results: a total of 39 science sources were quoted, averaging 0.36 per article, co mpared to 177 non scientist sources, averaging 1.64 per article. In coverage of climate change, a total of 192 science sources were quoted, an average of 0.79 per article, compared to 340 non scientist sources, an average of 1.39 per article. These results support Hypothesis 1, that scientists would be a minority of quoted sources. There were few correlations between the number of scientists quoted and the various frames (Table 3 5). The science frame was significantly positively correlated with number of s cientists quoted in all three controversies. Scientific uncertainty was significantly positively correlated with the number of scientists quoted the net band and reef fishery, but not climate change. There were no other patterns of significant correlations between scientists quoted and frame across controversies.

PAGE 50

50 Discussion Mass media framing of can have a strong influence on public understanding of unobtrusive issues such as coastal resource management controversies (Atwater et al. 1985; Lee 2004). The use of different sources can influence public perception as well, as successful claims makers can help define how the public understands a problem and influence the operating environment for communications campaigns (Gore and Knuth 2009). This study character ized the generic and science frame use three controversies and found that, while there was considerable variation in framing among the controversies, there were also common themes. Additionally, this study found that scientists were rarely used as sources compared to other types of claims makers. The implications of the findings are discussed below. Science a nd Generic Framing The first research question asked how the controversies were framed in terms of science frames and generic frames. The results show ed that the most common frames in all three controversies were conflict and economic consequences. The prevalence of these frames suggests that, across issues, journalists found conflict and economics to be either the most newsworthy or most important aspe cts of the controversies. The science frame was comparatively uncommon, appearing in significantly fewer than half of the articles about climate change and the net ban and approximately half of the reef fishery articles. The relative scarcity of science fr aming indicates that either science was not perceived as newsworthy, important, or interesting as other aspects of the controversies. However, the fact that the science frame, and to a lesser extent the scientific uncertainty frame, was consistently used i n each controversy suggests that they might be generic frames for coverage of natural resource management

PAGE 51

51 controversies, just as strategic framing is generic to political news coverage (Cappella and Jamieson 1997). Despite a large overlap in the use of fra mes, the overall framing was different for each controversy. For example, while conflict was the dominant frame in all three controversies, the relative frequency of the conflict frame varied. The conflict frame appeared in nearly every story about the net ban compared to less than half of the stories about climate change. In fact, the gap between the prevalence of the conflict frame and the next most common frame (which was economic consequences in all three controversies) differed greatly among the contro versies, as well. Media coverage of the net ban was much more dominated by the conflict frame than was coverage of either the reef fishery or climate change. The other frames were similarly unevenly used in the different controversies. The di fferences amon g controversies are also reflected in the multiple comparison measures, which showed that climate change coverage was framed much more widely than the other controversies. There was no significant difference in the prevalence of the top four frames in clim ate change, and there were only 3 Q groups with an average of 2.67 members in each group. This contrasts the findings for both the net ban and reef fishery controversies, which each had 4 Q groups and averaged 1.5 members per group. In other words, climate equal weight to more frames, than did the coverage of the other controversies. Turning to the use of the science frame, there was no consistent association between the science frame and the other frame s across controversies. For example, the science frame was highly significantly correlated with economic consequences in both

PAGE 52

52 the reef fishery and climate change controversies, but insignificantly correlated with the economic consequences frame in the net ban. The lack of consistent associations suggests that the use of the science frame might be story or issue specific rather than predictable across issues. The 63.7% article classification success rate in the discriminant function analysis indicates that t he two axes, interpreted as scientific uncertainty and conflict, were good but imperfect separators of the controversies. The differing coverage of the controversies may be because of intrinsic differences between them or may simply be a function of the is sue attention cycle, as described by Trumbo (1996). Trumbo found that, as climate change matured as an issue, media coverage of it changed, too. Each of the three controversies had different time horizons (several years for the net ban, several decades for climate change, and in between for the reef fishery), and differences in where they were in the maturation cycle may explain some of the differences in the way they were covered. pular understanding of the controversies. The net ban had relatively high levels of the conflict function compared to the other controversies and was relatively neutral on the scientific uncertainty function. This finding fits the narrative of the net ban (Martin 1997; Adams et al. 2000) that despite being a fisheries management controversy, science and management was overwhelmed in the public discourse by conflict. Similarly, the reef fishery scored higher in the scientific uncertainty function than did th e other conflicts. This finding confirms the popular narrative of the reef fishery controversy, in which the quality and use of science has come under repeated public criticism (Tomalin 2005).

PAGE 53

53 Coverage of climate change scored slightly negatively for each function, indicating a relative lack of conflict and scientific uncertainty. This finding is initially surprising, given that climate change is a controversial political issue. However, Boykoff (2007) noted that the use of false balance, defined as the giv ing of undue coverage to a minority position like climate skepticism, has declined in climate change coverage in recent years as the scientific consensus has strengthened. These results affirm ft out opinion pieces and editorials probably lessened the prevalence of climate uncertainty. The advantage of analyzing media use of generic frames is in the potential for theory building (De Vreese 2005). These findings suggest that the two axes of the d iscriminant function analysis, conflict vs. lack of conflict and scientific uncertainty vs. lack of uncertainty, may be a starting point for building theories about coverage of natural resource controversies. There is reason to believe that the two axes mi ght be informative in other controversies. For example, conflict has been shown to be an nce the presence of a generic conflict frame was not specifically discussed in those studies, and scientific uncertainty did not appear to be a significant part of the controversies, this classification scheme must be deemed preliminary and in need of furt her study. Science Frame and Scientists as Sources The second research question asked about the extent to which scientists were used as sources and how the prevalence of scientist sources varied by frame. This study found that scientists were the minority of sources quoted, as predicted by Hypothesis 1. This finding contrasts those of Corbett (1992) and Jacobson et al. (2012),

PAGE 54

54 who reported that scientist and government sources were the most frequently quoted sources in their systems. The discrepancy is like ly explained a prior study that showed that politicized or controversial issues tend to quote scientists less often than other scientists. A potential causal mechanism for the relative lack of scientist quotes in controversial issues is that highly contr oversial or politicized issues are more likely to attract claims makers on multiple sides of the issue, giving journalists a larger pool of that, as mountain lions bec ame more controversial, more claims makers arose, attempting to use mountain lions as a lever to make larger political points. Other than the science frame, there was no consistent association between frames use and the number of scientists quoted. This f inding suggests that, across controversies, scientists are only quoted incidentally with other frames and it may be difficult for scientists to get quoted among the increased competition among claims makers. Practical Implications These findings illustrate how difficult it is for outreach and communication personnel to use mass media as a method for communicating with stakeholders. The lack of consistent framing across issues means that science communicators might not ll issues. For example, an issue that is primarily covered in terms of conflict might require different communication than an issue

PAGE 55

55 primarily covered in terms of economics. The relative scarcity of science framing and scientists quoted means that the publi c might perceive the science related to an issue as less important than other aspects of the issue. Science may be diluted by other coverage, especially as issues become controversial and other potential claims makers compete for attention. The findings un derscore the importance of carefully planning media campaigns to maximize the few opportunities for science coverage in controversial issues while simultaneously looking outside of the media for additional chances to interact with stakeholders (Fazio et al 2001; Jacobson 2009). Future Studies As a first attempt to use generic frames to analyze coverage, this study raises more questions than it answers. Most notably, the frames in this study need to be analyzed across more controversies to see what patterns emerge. The discriminant function analysis needs to be repeated, as well, to see if the classification scheme holds. Finding a consistent, straightforward way to classify controversies might be extremely useful for outreach and communication, allowing com municators to better target their outreach. coverage. Does the audience receive the appropriate information if the science frame is used a minority of the time? This is a question that could be answered via experimental or survey work, and the answer could greatly inform communication strategy. If the amount of science coverage were inadequate for agency or outreach goals, then agencies or institutions interested in outreach would need to attempt to find other ways of reaching the public.

PAGE 56

56 Finally, the diversity of frame use across controversy merits further investigation. Diversity of generic frame use could be measured across additional controversies, perhaps using indices from the ecological literature such as the Shannon Weaver index. It's possible that framing diversity affects reader comprehension of or attitudes toward an issue. Regardless of whether these extensions prove fruitful, this study underscores nt about the suitability of generic frames for theory building. Limitations There are several limitations to this study. The first is that the conclusions, while theoretically and statistically sound, are based on studying three controversies in one state. Florida, generally, might limit the generalizability of the findings. Additionally, issues change over time, and this study only analyzes a snapshot of the coverage of t he reef fishery and climate change. Coverage of these issues may change, too, and the framing with it. Finally, the media analyzed, news stories published in newspapers, are only a part of the total media coverage of the issues. Other media influence publi c perceptions as well and may have been framed differently.

PAGE 57

57 Table 3 1. Percent present and absent frames by controversy. Frame Controversy Present (%) Absent (%) Conflict Net Ban 91.0 9.0 c Reef Fishery 74.1 25.9 c Climate Change 46.7 53.3 Eco nomic Consequences Net Ban 56.2 43.8 Reef Fishery 59.3 40.7 a Climate Change 44.3 55.7 a Human Interest Net Ban 40.4 59.6 a Reef Fishery 33.3 66.7 c Climate Change 32.8 67.2 c Science Net Ban 27.0 73.0 c Reef Fishery 52.8 47.2 Climate Change 39.3 60.7 c Morality Net Ban 14.6 85.4 c Reef Fishery 19.4 80.6 c Climate Change 18.0 82.0 c Scientific Uncertainty Net Ban 14.6 85.4 c Reef Fishery 49.1 50.9 Climate Change 8.2 91.8 c Note: Superscripts indicat e significant prese nce or absence beyond a binomial 50% random chance ( a =p<0.05, b =p<0.01, c= p<0.001). Net ban N=89, reef fishery n=108, climate change n=244.

PAGE 58

58 Table 3 2. Frames by relative dominance for each controversy. Controversy Frame Group(s) Ne t Ban Conflict A Economic Consequences B Human Interest C Science D Morality D Scientific Uncertainty D Reef Fishery Conflict A Economic Consequences B Human Interest C Science B Morality D Scientific Uncertainty B Climate Change Conf lict A Economic Consequences A Human Interest A, B Science A Morality B, C Scientific Uncertainty C Note: A grouped frames are more prevalent than B grouped frames, etc.

PAGE 59

59 Table 3 3. Standardized dimension coefficients for the discriminant functi on analysis Frame Dimension 1 Dimension 2 Conflict 0.08 1.08 Scientific Uncertainty 0.95 0.33 Human Interest 0.27 0.22 Morality 0.04 0.29 Economic Consequences 0.07 0.23 Science 0.12 0.26

PAGE 60

6 0 Table 3 4. Association between the science fra mes and other frames, as measured by the phi coefficient, stratified by controversy Frame Net Ban Reef Fishery Climate Change Scientific Uncertainty 0.11 0.48 c 0.04 Conflict 0.19 0.37 c 0.20 b Economic Consequences 0.08 0.35 c 0.31 c Human Interest 0.02 0.12 0.08 Morality 0.11 0.23 a 0.20 b Note: Superscripts indicat e statistical significance: a=p<0.05, b =p<0.01, c=p<0.001.

PAGE 61

61 Table 3 5. Point biserial correlation between the number of scientists quoted and frame presence, stratified by controvers y. Frame Net Ban Reef Fishery Climate Change Conflict 0.05 0.19 a 0.20 b Economic Consequences 0.02 0.13 0.23 c Human Interest 0.15 0.17 0.11 Morality 0.25 b 0.10 0.24 c Scientific Uncertainty 0.30 a 0.19 a 0.03 Science 0.35 c 0.31 c 0.77 c Note: S uperscripts indicat e statistical significance: a=p<0.05, b =p<0.01, c=p<0.001.

PAGE 62

62 CHAPTER 4 CLIMATE CHANGE AND COASTAL ENVIRONMENTAL RISK PERCEPTIONS Climate Risk Perceptions Climate change will likely have a dramatic and disproportionate effect on coastal r egions, exacerbating existing coastal environmental risks, including flooding, shoreline erosion, and habitat change, among other effects (IPCC 2007). Despite the potential impact of climate change and related environmental risks, climate outreach and comm unication has been ineffective (Wolf and Moser 2011). Risk perceptions are an important influence of the political context of policymaking (Slovic 1999; Leiserowitz 2006); understanding risk perceptions can be a key part of improving risk communication (Sl ovic 1987; Keller et al. 2006). However, our knowledge of climate related risk perception is still developing and there is substantial work to be done to clarify public perception of climate change risks. Surveys have shown that about 55% of Americans say they are "somewhat" or "very" worried about climate change and about 45% say they are "not very" or "not at all" worried (Leiserowitz et al. 2010). Climate change risk perceptions in the United States are influenced by a lack of accurate climate knowledge (Leiserowitz et al. 2010; Wolf and Moser 2011). However, climate change knowledge is negatively associated with perceived responsibility for climate change, perhaps because more informed people recognize the political will and collective action required to alter the course of climate change (Kellstedt et al. 2008). Part of the reason for the relative lack of climate concern is that climate related risks are perceived as affecting people who are temporally or geographically distant from the present day Unite d States (Leiserowitz 2005). Long term threats to distant

PAGE 63

63 people are often under valued compared to more immediate and salient threats (Stern 1992; CRED 2009). These factors conspire to minimize the perceived threat from climate change, which may partially explain why climate change is not a primary concern to many people in the United States. However, climate change is not necessarily a specific risk itself as much as a driver and catalyst of other risks. These effects may be particularly noted in coastal communities, where climate change may exacerbate sea level rise, heavy precipitation events (IPCC SREX 2012), marine fishery declines (Cheung et al. 2009), water pollution, and habitat loss (Tobey 2010). Specific coastal environmental risks may be more sal ient to coastal residents than general climate change risks, even if the environmental risks are climate related (Carlton and Jacobson, unpublished data). Thus, understanding coastal environmental risk perceptions and what drives those perceptions offers i mportant insight to risk communicators about specific coastal risks while also helping to clarify influences on general climate change risk perceptions. This study uses survey research to analyze perception of coastal environmental risks that are likely to be directly affected or exacerbated by climate change in Florida. The study is based on a conceptual model of risk perception (Figure 1) developed from prior research into the determinants of environmental risk perception, described below. Determinants of Risk Perception Early research on risk perception largely focused on rational choice models of risk, which presumed that individuals considered risks in an analytical manner, mentally calculating the odds and desirability of different risk outcomes and us ing those calculations as the basis for their risk perception (Lowenstein et al. 2001; Slovic et al. 2001; Leiserowitz 2006). Rational choice models imply that people make judgments

PAGE 64

64 eliberative system that encodes information in terms of words, numbers, and symbols (Epstein 1994; Lowenstein et al. 2001; Leiserowitz 2006). However, there is ample evidence that the rational larly well (Slovic et al. 2002). For example, studies that analyze risk perception from a purely cognitive, rational standpoint often find that laypeople have little understanding of the drivers, probabilities, and consequences required to rationally asses s a specific risk. et al. 1994; Read et al. 1994; Carlton and Jacobson unpublished data), which often reveal incomplete or inaccurate layperson understanding of risk. Instead of analytical, rational processing of risk, laypeople often process risks using their experiential processing system, which is affective and holistic and tends to encode information in terms of metaphors, stories, and images. Experiential processin g is typically non rational leading to emotion value and affect driven decisions and attitudes (Epstein 1994; Lowenstein et al. 2001; Slovic et al. 2001). In other words, laypeople are often reactive when perceiving risks. As a result, experts and lay people tend to think of risks in entirely different manners, which may be a significant factor in the relative ineffectiveness of many risk communications, particularly about complex topics such as climate change (Moser and Dilling 2011). Practically spea perception generally, and environmental risk perception specifically, is primarily a function of the characteristics of the individuals perceiving the risks and only secondarily a function of t he characteristics of the risks themselves (Sjberg 2000; Skimak and

PAGE 65

65 Dietz 2006). Additionally, the risk specific factors that influence risk perception, such as cont rol, and the morality of the risk over a risk rather than a dry calculation of the probabilities and consequences of a risk (Sandman 1987) While the precise drivers of risk perception may vary by system, there are several personal characteristics that often have been found to be important components of risk perception. Social trust : One primary influence of environmental risk perception appears to be social trust. In this context, social trust is a measure of the trust that an individual has in government agencies to manage a risk (Siegrist et al. 2000). Individuals with higher levels of social trust often perceive less risk than do individuals with lower levels of social trust. For example, tru st was a primary factor in public risk perception of a hypothetical Superfund waste site (Bord and O'Connor 1992). Similarly, social trust was an important factor in risk perceptions associated with the proposed nuclear waste repository in Yucca Mountain ( Flynn et al. 1992). The effects of social trust on risk perception go beyond waste issues: social trust has also been shown to affect risk perceptions of pesticides, nuclear power, artific ial sweetener (Siegrist et al. 2000), and prescribed burning (Vaske et al. 2007). While social trust has been found to be a significant component of risk perception in many studies, the finding is not unanimous: Sjberg (1998) found that social trust was not a significant factor in nuclear waste risk perception in Sweden.

PAGE 66

66 These prior studies of social trust and risk perceptions have been in systems with a direct, obvious connection between the risk and the government agency responsible for managing the risk. Since climate change is such a broad risk likely to have effects o n diverse parts of the environment and economy (IPCC 2007), climate related coastal environmental risks do not necessarily have such a clear cut connection. For example, it might not be obvious to laypeople which agency at which level of government is resp onsible for managing problems associated with drought or other, broad impacts. Environmental attitudes : People's environmental attitudes have been shown to affect their environmental risk perceptions. The New Ecological Paradigm (NEP; Dunlap et al. 2000) s cale and its predecessor, the New Environmental Paradigm (Dunlap et al. 1978) have become the most commonly used measures of environmental attitudes (Hawcroft and Milfont 2010). The NEP has been used in models of environmental values (Stern and Dietz 1994) models of environmental behavior (Stern et al. 1995), and in contingent valuation studies (Kotchen and Reiling 2000, Cooper et al. 2004). Several studies have shown that higher levels of environmental concern as rated by the NEP are associated with great er risk perceptions across a variety of ecological risks (Slimak and Dietz 2006), including climate change (Kellstedt et al. 2008). However, there is some controversy over what the NEP actually measures. The paper announcing the revised NEP suggested that the scale might measure five facets of an ecological worldview and that care should be exercised in treating the NEP as a single variable (Dunlap et al. 2000). Some studies have treated the NEP as a unidimensional scale of environmental concern (e.g., Coop er et al. 2004; Slimak and Dietz 2006; Kellstedt et al. 2008). Other studies have explicitly analyzed the

PAGE 67

67 dimensionality of the NEP (through factor analysis, structural equation modeling, and similar techniques) with inconsistent results (e.g., Shephard et al. 2009; Amburgey and Thoman 2012; Wu 2012). Risk salience : Another set of factors that can influence environmental risk perceptions can be loosely termed risk salience. Generally, risk salience is made up of two components: relevant prior experience and proximity to the risk. Relevant prior experience can directly influence risk perceptions. This was the case with the Chernobyl disaster, which increased the perceived risk of nuclear technology in Sweden, with the most affected people perceiving the great est risk from nuclear power (Drottz Sjoberg and Sjoberg 1990). In another study, experience with air pollution affected individual's climate change risk perception, though experience with flood did not (Whitmarsh 2008), a discrepancy that may be in part du e to differing environmental values. There is evidence that hurricanes may trigger climate risk perceptions, such as the increase in climate concern after hurricane Katrina in 2005 (Boykoff). Prior experience may influence risk perceptions because of the a vailability heuristic: people who have suffered from an environmental disaster may be more likely to recall that event when considering related environmental risks, increasing their risk perceptions (Keller et al. 2006). Similarly, actual or perceived (Gio rdano et al. 2011) proximity to a potential risk has been shown to affect risk perception, though this finding is not universal. Closeness to potential terrorist targets was associated with risk perceptions in a study in Michigan (Woods et al. 2008). Howev er, closeness to a major 1998 wildfire event in Florida did not change beliefs about prescribed burning (Jacobson et al. 2001). Sometimes the

PAGE 68

68 effects of proximity to an event differ by demographic group. For example, a November, 2001 national survey found that living within 100 miles of the World Trade Center (site of the September, 2001 terrorist attacks) increased terror risk perceptions, but only in a few demographic groups: men, adults, whites, and Republicans (Fischoff et al. 2003). The effects of prox imity on risk perception may be influenced by perceived benefits associated with the risk: those living closest to chemical manufacturing facilities in Houston perceived less risk from and had greater support for the chemical production industry, possibly because of the perceived economic benefits from the industry (Heath et al. 1998). Similarly, those living in the counties closest to the proposed Yucca Mountain repository perceived less risk from the site than those who lived farther away (Kunreuther et a l. 1990), although that difference may have been explained by demographic patterns. Demographic factors : Researchers have found several potential demographic influences of environmental risk perception. Of specific interest to this study of undergraduate s tudents are political affiliation and gender. Political affiliation may amplify or attenuate risk perceptions depending on the system being studied (Slovic 1999; Leiserowitz 2006 offers a specific environmental example). In addition, gender is an important component of risk: dozens of studies (`reviewed in Slovic 1999) have shown that females tend to perceive risks as greater than males do. Research Questions This study analyzes coastal environmental risk perceptions among Florida undergraduate students to address the following research questions: RQ1: How do Florida college students perceive climate change related coastal risks ?

PAGE 69

69 R Q2: What factors influence perceptions of risk? Methods In this study, risk perceptions were assessed and were used as a dependen t variable in a regression analysis with several independent variables in a model (Figure 4 1) derived from the general model described above: social trust, environmental attitudes, familiarity with relevant keystone events, proximity to the coast, politic al affiliation, and gender. The survey population consisted of 762 undergraduate students in two large, introductory classes at the University of Florida. The operationalization, measurement, and analysis of the variables is described below. Risk Perceptio ns Respondents were asked to rate their level of concern about 17 coastal environmental risks (Table 4 1). The risks were selected based on a model generated by Florida Sea Grant and a series of in depth key informant interviews with policymakers and techn ical staff in the coastal community of Crystal River, Florida (Carlton and Jacobson, unpublished data; Jacobson et al. 2012). Two of the risks (sea level rise and coastal erosion) were chosen because of their general applicability to Florida. The remainder were among the most commonly mentioned during the interviews. Respondents rated the risk items using the most least rating method, a technique that has been found to be an efficient way of avoiding end piling when rating lists of items (McCarty and Shrum 2000). Specifically, respondents were first presented with the list of risks that they would be asked to rate and were asked to choose which of the items is of most concern to them and which item is of least concern to them. Respondents were then asked to rate each of the items on a 10 point scale, with 1

PAGE 70

70 meaning "not at all concerned" and 10 meaning "strongly concerned". A principal component analysis with varimax rotation was performed on the risk questions to reduce the variables into interpretable compo nents. Factor scores were obtained using the regression method and were retained for use in later analyses. Retaining factor scores is superior to simply averaging the variables for each factor because factor scores optimally weight the observed variables for analysis (Stevens 1986). Social Trust Social trust was measured using several questions based on those developed by Vaske and Bright (2007). The questions were designed to assess respondents' trust of "Florida government officials" to effectively manag e coastal environmental risks. Respondents were asked "I trust Florida government officials to: (1) effectively manage coastal environmental risks, (2) provide the best available information on coastal environmental risks, (3) provide me with enough inform ation to decide what actions I should take regarding coastal environmental risks, (4) provide me with truthful information about coastal environmental risks, (5) provide me with timely information about coastal environmental risks". Responses were given on a 5 point Likert scale as above. As an additional indicator of social trust, respondents were asked "taking everything into consideration, how would you grade Florida government officials for handling coastal environmental risks?" Responses were given as a grade ranging from A to F. Environmental Attitudes The 15 question New Ecological Paradigm scale (Dunlap et al. 2000) was used

PAGE 71

71 principal components analysis was performed to assess the dimensionality of the scale before use in the regression model. Risk Salience The 2004 hurricane season, in which 4 different hurricanes (Charlie, Frances, Ivan, and Jeanne) made landfall in Florida, was used as a key prior coastal environmen tal risk event. Respondents were asked whether they lived in Florida during the 2004 hurricane season and the extent to which they were affected by the 2004 hurricanes, measured on a 1 Respondents who were not Florida residents in 2004 were assigned a score of 1. To assess general proximity to the coast, respondents were asked how far their permanent residence was from the coast: 1 mile or closer, 2 5 miles, 6 10 miles, 11 25 miles, or 26+ miles. Demographic Variables Respondents were asked to identify their political affiliation on a 7 point scale from "Strong Democrat" to "Strong Republican", with "Independent" as the middle value. Respondents also were asked their gender and age, thou gh age was omitted from the analysis because the vast majority (94.3%) of respondents were between the ages of 18 and 22 years old. Survey Administration and Data Analysis Prior to administration, a pilot test was performed to refine the instrument. Twenty three University of Florida undergraduates of similar demographics to the target population and 6 adults took the survey during the pilot test. Their feedback was incorporated prior to administrating the survey, causing us to revise the wording on the soc ial trust questions and several of the risk perception questions.

PAGE 72

72 The survey was administered electronically via SurveyMonkey to 762 students in two general education undergraduate classes at the University of Florida. Respondents were given electronic pre notification and several in class reminders about the survey. Results Response Rate A total of 558 completed surveys were received for a 73.2% response rate. A nonresponse check showed that the gender ratio of respondents was similar to the gender ratio o f nonrespondents. Additionally, the gender ratio and political affiliations were similar between the two classes surveyed. As a result, no post hoc weighting was applied. Risk Perceptions The average score for the risk items was 6.75 on a scale of 1 10. Re spondents were most concerned (i.e., a score of over 7 on a 1 10 scale) about drinking water loss (M=7.81, SD=2.20), beach loss (M=7.40, SD=2.03), property damage from hurricanes (M=7.34, SD=2.17), water contamination from septic tanks (M=7.31, SD=2.08), i ncreasing drought (M=7.21, SD=2.04), and climate change (M=7.13, SD=2.20). Respondents were least concerned (i.e., a score of less than 6 on a 1 10 scale) about land plant loss (M=5.91, SD=2.20), aquatic plant loss (M=5.77, SD=2.26), invasive plants (M=5.5 4, SD=2.34), and tourism declines (M=5.36, SD=2.54) (Table 4 1). For the risk data, three principal components explaining 65.29% of the variance were retained based on evaluation of a scree plot. The components were interpreted as physical environment risk s (M=7.17, SD=1.79), economic risks (M=6.66, SD=1.55), and biological risks (M=5.98, SD=1.88). All three components had acceptable internal consistency (Table 4 1). Three variables loaded highly on two or more components and

PAGE 73

73 were therefore droppe d from the analysis (Stevens 198 6): septic tank contamination, storm surge, and coastal erosion. Social trust The social trust indicators had high internal consistency (alpha = 0.81), and were averaged into a social trust index. The average social trust index score was 3.09 (SD = 0.78) out of 5, representing a moderate level of trust in Florida government officials to manage coastal risks. Environmental Attitudes The 15 item New Ecological Paradigm (NEP) scale had strong internal consistency (alpha = 0.80). The avera ge NEP score was 3.44 on a 5 point scale, indicating mild to moderate pro environmental attitudes among respondents. To address the concerns about dimensionality, a principal components analysis was performed. Thirteen of the 15 components loaded highl unidimensional scale. Demographics The majority of respondents (58.6%) were female. The average respondent was slightly Republican, rat ing themselves 3.98 (SD=1.62) on the 7 point political affiliation scale. Most (93%) respondents have lived in Florida for at least the last 5 years. Regression Model Each of the retained risk factor components (physical environment risks, economic risks, and biological risks) was used as a dependent variable in a separate multiple regression analysis (Table 4 2). The independent variables included social trust, the New Ecological Paradigm (NEP) scale, the extent to which respondents were affected

PAGE 74

74 by the 20 04 hurricanes, coast, models were significant at the p<0.001 level and visual inspection of residual plots suggeste d a normal residual variance structure. The NEP score was the biggest predictor of physical risk perception (B=0.57, p<0.001), with respondents expressing greater environmental concern also perceiving greater risk. Gender also was a significant predictor o f physical risk perception (B=0.20, p<0.05), with females perceiving greater risk than males. Political affiliation significantly predicted physical risk perception (B= 0.13, p<0.001), with more Democrat leaning respondents perceiving greater risk. Gender was the largest predictor of economic risk perception (B=0.26, p<0.05), with females again perceiving greater risk than males. NEP score was also a significant predictor (B= 0.20, p<0.05), with people who expressed less ecological concern perceiving greate r economic risk. Political affiliation also predicted economic risk perception (B=0.12, p<0.001), with more Republican respondents perceiving greater economic risk. NEP score was the only significant predictor of biological risk perception (B=0.37, p<0.00 1): those who had greater environmental concern perceived greater risk. Discussion environmental risk perceptions as much as or more than the characteristics of the risks themselves (Slimak and Dietz 2006). This study analyzed risk perceptions in undergraduates at the University of Florida and found that, though many proposed

PAGE 75

75 environmental risk perceptions were strongly influenced by attitudinal factors such as environmental attitudes and political affiliation. Risk Perceptions The first research question identified specific risk perceptions. In general, respondents were concerned about all 17 risks: each the midpoint on the concern scale. This may in part result from the potential for a survey priming effect. Practically, many of the highest concerns half of those rated above 7 on the 10 point scale were related to drinking water: drinking water loss, water contamination, and drought. The public concern for water quality and quantity is unsurprising in a coastal state like Florida. There are apparent similarities among some of the most highly rated risks. Both the water risks and property damage from hurricanes, which together are 4 of the 6 hazards that rated over 7, might be considered more immediate, personal threats to the respondents, especially compared to the items of least concern, including tourism declines, spread of invasive species, and aquatic and terrestrial plant population declines. This difference is also found in the general categories of risk identified by the factor analysis: respondents were more concerned about risks related to the physical environment and property than they were risks to the local flora and fauna. The ordering of concern for the different risk categories, and roughly the individual risk items, reflects prior research that individuals perceive salient, local, and more immediate risks of mor e concern than risks that are less personal (Stern 1992; CRED 2009). However, the connection between risk immediacy and risk perception was uneven. Climate change, generally considered by Americans to be a long term risk that will affect others (CRED 2009 ), was the sixth highest rated risk, averaging 7.12 on the 10

PAGE 76

76 point scale. This finding reflects prior nationwide surveys in which most Americans 2010). Practically, the ris k perception results illustrate specific concerns of Florida efforts in an attempt to achieve better results. Focusing a climate change message to the physical environment o r water supply risks, for example, might improve message salience for coastal residents, helping to improve ineffective climate related outreach (Wolf and Moser 2011). Influences o n Risk Perception The second research question was concerned with which fact ors influence risk perception. The regression analysis revealed that risk perceptions were most influenced their gender, and their political affiliation. Environmental atti tudes : Environmental attitudes were, in aggregate, the largest determinant of risk perception. Pro environmental attitudes (i.e., higher NEP scores) were positively associated with greater physical and biological risk perception, and negatively associated with greater economic risk perception. These findings affirm prior research showing that environmental risk perception is related to environmental attitudes (Slimak and Dietz 2006; Kellstedt et al. 2008). Additionally, these findings go beyond prior resear ch by showing that specific environmental risk perceptions, in addition to concerns about general risks like global warming, are influenced by environmental attitudes.

PAGE 77

77 The fact that the categories of risk were differently influenced by environmental attitu des illustrates the wide ranging effects of climate change. Even those who are not environmentally concerned (i.e., have a low NEP score) showed concern about some climate related hazards. These findings suggest an opportunity for climate outreach and pers onnel to find common ground with those who might not heed a climate change message and provide a starting point for the background and audience research that's a critical part of any communication campaign (Jacobson 2009). Political affiliation : Political affiliation was an important driver of risk perception for both physical environmental risks, where more Democrat leaning respondents perceived greater risks, and economic risks, where more Republican leaning respondents perceived greater risks. Prior rese arch has shown that Republican leaning individuals tend to perceive climate risks as lower (Leiserowitz 2005). These findings add nuance by showing that while Republican affiliation is negatively associated with the risks of climate change to the physical environment, Republican affiliation is associated with greater concern for property and economy related risks of climate change. The challenge for risk managers is to either make the connection between the different risks and climate change or find adapti ve or mitigative strategies that appeal to these concerns. Social trust : Social trust was not a significant predictor of risk perception in any of the risk categories. This lack of predictive power is this model contrasts with prior research about the inf luence of social trust on risk perceptions related to a variety of risks, including nuclear waste repositories (Flynn et al. 1992); Superfund waste sites

PAGE 78

78 et al. 2000); and prescribed burning (Vaske et al. 2008). While prior research has shown that students may already have developed social trust (Sjberg 1998), the process of social trust formation requires further examination, which may help clarify these results. Oth er studies also have found that social trust is not a factor in risk perception explanation power of trust depends on how it is operationalized. An unspecified measure of social trust (e.g., general social trust in government in all situations) might testing, referred to specific definitions of trust (i.e., government management of coas tal risks, government provision of information about coastal risks, etc.) and a relatively difficulty in relating social trust to climate risk perception. Climate related risks differ from risks related to, for example, prescribed burning or artificial sweeteners in that climate related risks are inherently multifactorial and are not clearly the responsibility of any particular local or federal government agency. Given thi s fact, it is possible that no specific operationalization of social trust would explain the variance in risk perception across the diversity of climate related risks. Risk salience : Neither prior experience with hurricanes nor distance of residence from t he coast were significant predictors of risk perception in any of the three case the 2004 hurricanes, contrasts with some previous work (e.g., nuclear power studied by Drottz Sjberg and Sjberg 1990; air pollution studied Whitmarsh 2008), but

PAGE 79

79 not all (e.g., flood experience studied Whitmarsh 2008). There are several possible explanations for this discrepancy. One is that the 2004 hurricanes are not an appropriate prior event, either because hurricanes are such a concern throughout Florida that even those who were not affected by the 2004 hurricanes are still worried about hurricanes generally, reducing the predictive power of the 2004 events. Similarly, enough time may have passed that the 2004 hurricanes are no longer cognitively related risk perceptions. Finally, climate change encompasses many types of risk, so hurricanes might not cause an availability heuristic eff ect beyond hurricane related risks. The irrelevance of proximity as a determinant of risk perception is somewhat surprising given that several recent studies found proximity to be important (Woods et al. 2008; Giordano et al. 2011). One potential explanati on is that the coast may be beaches and ocean destinations. A similar study of coastal environmental risk perceptions in a state with less coastline might yield differ ent results. This study built on prior, general studies of climate risk perception by analyzing perceptions of specific, climate related coastal environmental risks. It is one of the first papers to look at specific climate related risks rather than taking a more general approach. The findings showed that climate related coastal environmental risks are diverse, and may be difficult to analyze as a monolithic unit. Categorizing the risks, in this case physical environmental risks, economic risks, and biologi cal risks, allowed for a more nuanced analysis and understanding of the various drivers of risk perception. By

PAGE 80

80 focusing on the most salient categories, in this case physical environmental risks, risk managers might design more effective communications. Fac tors such as environmental attitudes or political affiliation were significant predictors of risk perception, supporting the notion that people tend to perceive risk experientially rather than analytically. The fact that people often do not consider risks analytically is important information for risk communicators as it serves as a reminder that simply providing accurate information about risks is not enough (Wolf and Moser 2011). Limitations There are several limitations to the study. First, the responden ts were college students, so these findings may not generalize to the population as a whole. Second, although the identified risks are believed to be generally applicable to many Florida communities, the risks might be irrelevant or confusing to some of th e respondents, affecting their responses.

PAGE 81

81 Figure 4 1. Conceptual model of coastal environmental risk perception. Items in bold were tested in the regression analysis.

PAGE 82

82 Table 4 1. Risk items divided into scales based on a principal components analysis. Risk item Mean SD Factor loading Physical environment risks 7.17 1.79 Drinking water loss 7.81 2.20 0.78 Drought 7.21 2.03 0.82 Climate change 7.12 2.20 0.87 Extreme temperatures 6.92 2.12 0.83 Sea level rise 6.83 2.17 0.74 Economic risks 6.66 1.55 Beach loss 7.41 2.02 0.48 Prop erty damage from hurricanes 7.34 2.17 0.57 Property insurance increases 6.72 2.16 0.85 Property value declines 6.48 2.30 0.88 Tourism declines 5.36 2.54 0.50 Biological risks 5.98 1.88 Fish population declines 6.72 2.11 0.65 Land plant population declines 5.91 2.20 0.91 Aquatic plant population declines 5.77 2.26 0.92 Spread of invasive plant species 5.54 2.34 0.75 Risks dropped from analysis Contamination from septic tanks 7.31 2.08 Storm surge 6.9 8 2.05 Coastal erosion 6.76 1.99

PAGE 83

83 Table 4 2. Multiple r egression models by risk category with risk perception as the dependent variable. Independent variable Model 1: Physical environment risk Model 2: Economic risk Model 3: Biological risk Soc ial Trust 0.06 0.12 0.1 New Ecological Paradigm 0.57 c 0.20 a 0.37 c 2004 Hurricane effects 0.03 0.01 0.01 Proximity 0.01 0.04 0.06 Political affiliation 0.13 c 0.12 c 0.04 Gender (dcv) 0.20 a 0.26 a 0.074 Adjusted r2 0.22 0.20 0.21 N 491 491 4 91 Note: Superscripts indicat e statistical significance: a=p<0.05 c=p<0.001.

PAGE 84

84 CHAPTER 5 CONCLUSION The three studies in this dissertation took different approaches to understanding how the public receives, processes, and perceives information about contr oversial coastal environmental and natural resource issues. The findings expand on prior practical and theoretical work and suggest ways to improve outreach, communication, and policymaking on these difficult issues. The mental models study built on prior mental models work and found that there were substantial differences in the way that experts and nonexperts perceived climate change related hazards. The results also showed that there are a number of less controversial, climate related topics that climat e outreach, communication, and policymaking personnel can focus on to achieve their goals while avoiding some of the public climate change controversy. This difference is critical to consider in the development of outreach, communication, and policy. The content analysis used generic and science frame analysis to reveal that newspaper coverage of coastal natural resource management controversies, which is a critical source of public knowledge about the controversial topics, focuses largely on conflict and economics at the expense of scientific knowledge. This finding illustrates the difficulty in using mass media for outreach and communication, as the message might be diluted by other concerns. Additionally, education, outreach, and communication personnel need to carefully analyze each issue to tailor their programs appropriately, because media coverage may vary drastically by issue. Finally, the coastal risks survey built on prior, general studies of climate risk perception by analyzing perceptions of spe cific, climate related coastal environmental

PAGE 85

85 risks. The findings showed that climate related coastal environmental risks are diverse, and may be difficult to analyze as a monolithic unit. Categorizing the risks as physical environmental risks, economic ris ks, and biological risks, allowed for a more nuanced analysis and understanding of the various drivers of risk perception. Survey respondents were primarily concerned about risks to the physical environment, especially the water supply, followed by risks t o the economy and risks to the biological components of the environment. The findings also revealed that factors such as environmental attitudes or political affiliation were significant predictors of risk perception, supporting the notion that people tend to perceive risk experientially rather than analytically. The fact that people often do not consider risks analytically is important information for risk communicators as it serves as a reminder that simply providing accurate information about risks is in sufficient for changing behavior. Together, these three studies illustrate the challenge of expert nonexpert communication. Experts and nonexperts think about issues differently, often use different communication channels, and evaluate risks differently. A s coastal environmental issues become more pressing, the ability to successfully overcome these difficulties becomes more important. The work in this dissertation is a significant theoretical and practical step in that process.

PAGE 86

86 LIST OF REFERENCES Adams, C., S. Jacob, and S. Smith. 2000. What happened after the net ban? Retrieved 1 January 2012: h ttp://edis.ifas.ufl.edu/fe123 Ader, C. 1995. A longitudinal study of agenda setting for the issue of environment al pollution. Journalism Quarterly 72(2): 300 311. Adger, W. N., S. Dessai M. Goulden, M. Hulme, I. Lorenzoni, D. R. Nelson, L. O. Naess, J. Wolf, and A. Wreford. 2009. Are there social limits to adaptation to climate change? Climatic Change 93:335 354. A mburgey, J. W., and D. B. Thoman. 2012. Dimensionality of the new ecological paradigm. Environment and Behavior 44 : 235 256. Atwater, T., M. Salwen, and R. Andersen. 1985. Media agenda setting with the environmental issues. Journalism Quarterly 62(2) : 393 3 97. Audit Bureau of Circulations. 2012. US Newspaper Circulations. Retrieved 12 June 2012: h ttp://abcas3.accessabc.com/ecirc/newstitlesearchus.asp Berelson, B. 1952. Content Analysis in Communication Research. New York: Free Press. Borberg, J., L. Jodice, M. Harte, and P. Corcoran. 2009. An analysis of a survey of Oregon coast decision makers regarding climate change Oregon Sea G rant Publication ORESU S 09 001, Corvallis, Oregon. Bor d, R. J., A. Fisher United States and international perspectives. Climate Research 11:75 84. Bord, R. J. and R. waste site. R isk Analysis 12: 411 416. Bostroom, A., B. Fischhoff and M. G. Morgan. 1992. Characterizing mental models of hazardous processes: A methodology and an application to radon. Journal of Social Issues 48:85 100. Bostrom, A., M. G. Morgan, and B. Fischoff 199 4. What do people know about climate change? 2. Mental model studies of lay opinion leaders. Risk Analysis 14:959 970. Boykoff, M. T. 2007. Flogging a dead norm? Newspaper coverage of anthropogenic climate change in the United States and United Kingdom fro m 2003 to 2006. Area 39: 470 481. Bryant, J., and M. B. Oliver, eds. 2009. Media effects: Advances in theory and research New York: Routledge.

PAGE 87

87 Capella, J., and K. H. Jamieson. 1997. Spiral of cynicism: The press and the public good. Oxford: Oxford Universi ty Press. Carvalho, A. 2007. Ideological cultures and media discourses on scientific knowledge: re reading news on climate change. Public Understanding of Science 16: 223 243. Center for Research on Environmental Decisions (CRED). 2009. The psychology of cl imate change communication: A guide for scientists, journalists, educators, political aides, and the interested public New York. Cheung, W. W. L., V. W. Y. Lam J. L. Sarmiento, K. Kearney, R. Watson, and D. Pauly. 2009. Projecting global marine biodivers ity impacts under climate change scenarios. Fish and Fisheries 10:235 251. Coastal States Organization (CSO). 2008. The role of coastal zone management programs in adaptation to climate change Washington, D.C.: Second annual report of the Coastal States O ate Change Workgroup, Washingt on, D. C. Coleman, F. C., W. F. Figueira, J. S. Ueland, and L. B. Crowder. 2004. The impact of United States recreational fisheries on marine fish populations. Science 305:1958 1960. Conover, W. J. 1998. Prac tical nonparametric statistics, 3rd ed. Hoboken : Wiley. Cooper, P., G. L. Poe, and I. J. Bateman. 2004. The structure of motivation for contingent values: A case study of lake water quality improvement. Ecological Economics 50: 69 82. Corbett, J. 1992. Rura l and urban newspaper coverage of wildlife: conflict, community and bureaucracy. Journalism Quarterly 69: 929 937. Cowan, J. H., Jr., C. B. Grimes, W. F. Patterson, III, C. J. Walters, A. C. Jones, W. J. Lindberg, D. J. Sheehy, W. E. Pine, III, J. E. Powers M. D. Campbell, K. C. Lindeman, S. L. Diamon d, R. Hilborn, H. T. Gibson, and K. A. Rose. 2010. Red snapper management in the Gulf of Mexico: Science or faith based? Reviews in Fish Biology and Fisheries 21:187 204. Cox, R. 2006. Environmental communicat ion and the public sphere Thousand Oaks, Ca: Sage Publications. Cox, P., J. Niewhner, N. Pidgeon, S. Gerrard, B. Fischhoff, and D. Riley. 2003. The use of mental models in chemical risk protection: Developing a generic workplace methodology. Risk Analysi s 23:311 324. Cramer, H. 1946. Mathematical methods of statistics. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

PAGE 88

88 D e Vreese, C. H. 2005. News framing: Theory and typology. Information Design Journal & Document Design 13: 51 62. Dirikx, A., and D. Gelders. 2010. Ide ologies overruled? An explorative study of the link between ideology and climate change reporting in Dutch and French newspapers. Environmental Communication 4: 190 205. Dotson, D. M., S. K. Jacobson, L. L. Kaid, and J. S. Carlton. 2012. Media coverage of c limate change in Chile: A content analysis of conservative and liberal newspapers. Environmental Communication 6:64 81. Drottz Sjberg, B., and L. Sjberg. 1990. Risk perception and worries after the Chernobyl accident. Journal of Environmental Psychology 10:135 149. Dunlap, R. E., and K. D. Van Liere. 1978. proposed measuring instrument and preliminary results. Journal of Environmental Education 9: 10 19. Dunlap, R. E., K. D. Van Liere, A. G. Mertig, R. E. Jones. 2000. Me asuring endorsement of the new ecological paradigm: A revised NEP scale. Journal of Social Issues 56: 425 442. Entman, R. M. 1993. Framing: Toward clarification of a fractured paradigm. Journal of Communication 41:51 58. Epstein, S. 1994. Integration of the cognitive an d the psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist 49: 709 724. Eyal, C. H., J. P. Winter, and W. F. DeGeorge. 1981. The concept of time frame in agenda setting. In Mass Communication Yearbook, Vol. 2, ed ited by G. C. Wilhoit, 212 218. Beve rly Hills: Sage. Fazio, J. R., D. L. Gilbert, and D. Lowe. 2001. Public relations and communications for natural resource managers Dubuque, IA: Kendall Hunt Publishing. Fischoff, B., R. M. Gonzalez, D. A. Small, J. S. Lerner. 2003. Judged terror risk and proximity to the World Trade Center. The Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 26: 137 151. Fiske, J., and S. E. Taylor. 1991. Social cognition. New York : McGraw Hill. Flynn, J., W. B urns, C. K. Mertza, and P. Slovic. 1992. Trust as a determinant of opposition to a high level radioactive waste repository: Analysis of a structural model. Risk Analysis 12: 417 429. Gamson, W. 1999. Beyond the science versus advocacy distinction. Contemporary Sociology 28: 23 26.

PAGE 89

89 Gamson, W. A. and A. Modigliani. 1987. The changing cul ture of affirmative action. Research in Political Sociolo g y 3: 137 177. Gans, H. 1979. The CBS Evening News, NBC Nightly News, Newsweek, and Time. New York: Vintage. Gentner, D. and A. Stevens. 1983. Mental models. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum. Gifford, R. 2011. The dragons of inaction: Psychological barriers that limit climate change mitigation and adaptation. American Psychologist 66:290 302. Giordano, A., S. Anderson, and X. He. 2010. How near is near? The distance perceptions o f residents of a nuclear emergency planning zone. Environmental Hazards 9: 167 182. Gore, M. L., W. F. Siemer, J. E. Shanahan, D. Schuefele,, and D. J. Decker. 2005. Effects on risk perception of media coverage of a black bear related human fatality. Wildli fe Society Bulletin 33:507 516. Gore, M. L., and B. A. Knuth. 2009. Mass media effect on the operating environment of a wildlife related risk communication campaign. Journal of Wildlife Management 73: 1407 1413. Hardisty, D. J., E. J. Johnson, and E. U. Web er. 2010. A dirty word or a dirty world? Attribute framing, political affiliation, and query theory. Psychological Science 21:86 92. Hawcroft, L. J., and T. L. Milfont. 2010. The use (and abuse) of the new environmental paradigm scale over the last 30 year s: A me ta analysis. Journal of Environmental Psychology 30: 143 158. Heath, R. L., S. Seshadri, and J. Lee. 1998. Risk communication: A two community analysis of proximity, dread, trust, involvement, uncertainty, openness/accessibility, and knowledge on su pport/opposition toward chemical companies. Journal of Public Relations Research 10: 35 56. Henderson, B. J. and B. T. Maguire. 2000. Three lay mental models of disease inheritance. Social Science & Medicine 50:293 301. Intergovernmental Panel on Climate C hange (IPCC). 2007. Climate change 2007: Impacts, adaptation and vulnerability Contribution of Working Group II to the Fourth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change New York: Cambridge University Press. Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC). 2012. Managing the risks of extreme events and disasters to advance climate change adaptation New York : Cambridge University Press.

PAGE 90

90 Iyengar, S. 1991. Is anyone responsible? How television frames political issues Chicago : Univer sity of Chicago Press. Jacobson, S. K. 2009. Communication skills for conservation professionals, 2 nd edition. Washington, D.C.: Island Press. Jacobson, S. K., C. Langin, J. S. Carlton, and L. L. Kaid. 2012. Content analysis of newspaper coverage of the Fl orida panther. Conservation Biology 26: 171 179. Jacobson, S. K., M. McDuff, and M. Monroe. 2006. Conservation Education and Outreach Techniques. London: Oxford University Press. Jacobson, S. K., M. C. Monroe, and S. Marynowski. Fire at the wildland inte rface: The influence of experience and mass media on public knowledge, attitudes, and behavioral intentions. Wildlife Society Bulletin 29: 929 937. Kaplan, S. 2000. Human nature and environmentally responsible behavior. Journal of Social Issues 56:491 508. Keller, C., M. Siegrist, and H. Gutscher. 2006. The role of the affect and availability heuristics in risk communication. Risk Analysis 26: 631 639. Kellstedt, P. M., S. Zahran, and A. Vedlitz. 2008. Personal efficacy, the information environment, and attit udes toward global warming and climate change in the United States. Risk Analysis 28: 113 126. Krippendorff, K. 2004. Content analysis: An introduction to its methodology, 2nd edition. Thousand Oaks, Ca: Sage Publications. Kotchen, M. J., and S. D. Reiling. 2000. Environmental attitudes, motivations, and contingent valuation of nonuse values: a case study involving endangered species. Ecological Economics 32: 93 107. Kunreuther, H., D. Easterling, W. Desvousges, P. Slovic. 1990. Public attitudes toward siting a high level nuclear waste repository in Nevada. Risk Analysis 10: 469 484. Lacy, S., D. Riffe, S. Stoddard, H. Martin, and K. Chang 2001. Sample size for newspaper content analysis in multi year studies. Journalism and M ass Communication Quarterly 78: 83 6 845 Lee, G. analytical model of media agenda setting effects. International Communication Gazette 66: 151 166. Leiserowitz, A. A. 2005. American risk perceptions: Is climate change dangerous? Risk Analysis 25:1433 1442.

PAGE 91

91 Leiserowitz, A. 2006. Climate change risk perception and policy preferences: The role of affect, imagery, and values. Climatic Change 77: 45 72. Leiserowitz, A., N. Smith, and J. R. Marlon. 2010. wledge of climate change New Haven, CT: Yale Project on Climate Change Communication. Lowe, T. D. and I. Lorenzoni. 2007. Danger is all around: Eliciting expert perceptions for managing climate change through a mental models approach. Global Environmental Change 17:131 146. Lowenstein, G. F., E. U. Weber, C. K. Hsee, and N. Welch. 2001. Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin 127:267 286. Maher, T. M. 2001. Framing: An emerging paradigm or a phase of agenda setting. In Framing public life: Perspectives on media and our understanding of the social world edited by S. D. Reese, O. H. Gandy, and A. E. Grant, 83 94. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Martin, W. process and limit sub ject matter. Florida State University Law Review 25: 57 86. McCarty, J. A., and L. J. Shrum. 2000. The measurement of personal values in survey research: A test of alternative rating procedures. Public Opinion Quarterly 64: 271 298. McCombs, M., and G. Estra da. 1997. The news media and the pictures in our heads. In Do the media govern? Politicians, voters, and reporters in America, edited by S. Iyengar and R. Reeves, 237 247. Thousand Oak, CA: Sage. McCombs, M. E., and D. L. Shaw. 1972. The agenda setting fun ction of mass media. Public Opinion Quarterly 36: 176 185. Miller, M. M., and B. P. Riechert. 2001. The spiral of opportunity and frame resonance: Mapping the issue cycle in news and public discourse. In Framing public life: Perspectives on media and our un derstanding of the social world edited by S. D. Reese, O. H. Gandy, and A. E. Grant, 107 121. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Morgan, M. G., B. Fischh off A. Bostrom and C. J. Atman. 2002. Risk Communication: A Mental Models Approach. New York: Cambridge U niversity Press. Moser, S. C. and L. Dilling. 2004. Making climate hot: Communicating the urgency and challenge of global climate change. Environment 46:32 46. Moser, S. C. and L. Dilling. 2011. Communicating climate change: Clo sing the science action gap In The Oxford Handbook of Climate Change and Society ed ited by J. S. Dryzek, R. B. Norgaard and D. Schlosberg, 161 174. New York: Oxford University Press.

PAGE 92

92 Muter, B. A., M. I. Gore, and S. J. Riley. 2009. From victim to perpetrator: Evolution of risk fra mes related to human cormorant conflict in the Great Lakes. Human Dimensions of Wildlife 14: 366 379. Niewhner, J., P. Cox, S. Gerrard, and N. Pidgeon. 2004. Evaluating the efficacy of a mental models approach for improving occupational chemical risk prote ction. Risk Analysis 24:349 361. Nisbet, M. C. 2009. Communicating climate change: Why frames matter for public engagement. Environment 51: 12 23. Norman, D. A. 1983. Some observations on mental models. In: Mental Models edited by D. Gentner and A. L. Stev ens, 7 14. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Parkinson, R. W. and T. McCue. 2011. Assessing municipal vulnerability to predicted sea level rise: City of Satellite Beach, Florida. Climatic Change 107:203 223. Patton, M. 2002. Qualitative Research and Evaluation Meth ods. 3 rd edition. London: Sage Publications Price, V., and D. Tewksbury. 1997. News values and public opinion: A theoretical account of media priming and framing. In Progress in Communication Sciences: Advances in Persuasion 13 edited by G. Barnet and F. J. Boster, 248 269. Greenwich, CT: Ablex Publishing Corporation. Read, D., A. Bostrom, M. G. Morgan, B. Fischoff, and T. Smuts. 1994. What do people know about climate change? 2. Survey studies of educated laypeople. Risk Analysis 14:971 982. Sanchez, P. M 1974. The unequal group size problem in discriminant analysis. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 2: 629 633. Sandman, P. M. 1987. Risk communication: Facing public outrage. EPA Journal 13:21 22. Scheufele, D. A., and D. Tewksbury. 2007. Framing agenda setting, and priming: The evolution of three media effects models. Journal of Communication 57: 9 20. Scott, W. A. 1955. Reliability of content analysis: The case of nominal scale coding. Public Opinion Quarterly 19: 321 325. Semetko, H. A., and P. M. Valkenburg. 2000. Framing Eur op ean politics: A content analysis of press and television news. Journal of Communication 50: 93 109. Shephard, K., S. Mann, N. Smith, and L. Deaker. 2009. Benchmarking the environmental values and attitudes of students in Ne compulsory education. Environmental Education Research 15: 571 587

PAGE 93

93 Siegrist, M., G. Cvetkovich, and C. Roth. 2000. Salient value similarity, social trust, and risk/benefit perception. Risk Analysis 20: 353 362. Siemer, W. F., D. J. Decker, and J. Shanahan. 2007. Media frames for black bear management stories during issue emergence in New York. Human Dimensions of Wildlife 12: 89 100. Sjberg, L. 1998. Worry and risk perception. Risk Analysis 18: 85 93. Sjberg L. 2000. Factors in risk percept ion. Risk Analysis 20: 1 11. Slimak, M.W., and T. Dietz. 2006. Personal values, beliefs, and ecological risk perception. Risk Analysis 26: 1689 1705. Slovic, P. 1987. Perception of risk. Science 236: 280 285. Slovic, P. 1999. Trust, emotion, sex, politics, an d science: Surveying the risk assessment battlefield. Risk Analysis 19: 689 701. Slovic, P., M. Finucane, E. Peters, and D. G. MacGregor. 2002. The affect heuristic. In Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment, edited by T. Gilovich, D. G riffin, and D. Kahneman 397 420. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Smith, N. 2006. The case of the commercial fisheries constitutional net ban amendment in Florida: An illustration of the impact of special interest associations on institutional chang e Doctoral Dissertation, The Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida. Spector, M., and J. Kitsuse. 1977. Constructing social problems Menlo Park, CA: Cummings. Spence, A. and N. Pidgeon. 2010. Framing and communicating climate change: The effects of distance and outcome frame manipulations. Global Environmental Change 20:656 667. Sterman, J. D. 2008. Risk communication on climate: Mental models and mass balance Science 322:532 533. Sterman, J. D. and Booth Sweeney, L. 2007. Understanding public co mplacency about matter. Climatic Change 80:213 238. Stern, P. C. 1992. Psychological dimensions of global environmental change. Annual Review of Psychology 43:269 302. Stern, P C., and T. Dietz. 1994. The value basis of environmental concern. Journal of Social Issues 50: 65 84.

PAGE 94

94 Stern, P. C., T. Dietz, and G. A. Guagnano. 1995. The new environmental paradigm in social psychological perspective. Environment and Behavior 27: 723 745 Stevens, J. 1986. Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates. Tankard, J. 2001. The empirical approach to the study of media framing. In Framing public life: Perspectives on media and our understa nding of the social world ed ited by S. Reese, O. Gandy and A. Grant, 95 106. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Tewksbury, D., and D. A. Scheufele 2009. News framing theory and research. In Media effects: Advances in theory and research, edited by J. Bryant and M. B. Oliver, 17 33. New York : Routledge. Titus, J. G., D. E. Hudgens D. L. Trescott M. Crahan, W. H. Nuckols C. H. Hershner, J. M. Kassakian, C. J. Linn, P. G. Merritt, T. M. McCue, J. F. J. Tanski and J. Wang. 2009. State and local government s plan for development of most land vulnerable to rising sea level along the US Atlantic coast. Environmental Research Letters 4 :044008 Tobey, J., P. Rubinoff, D. Robadue, Jr., G. Ricci, R. Volk, J. Furlow, and G. Anderson. 2010. Practicing coastal adapta tion to cl imate change: Lessons from inte grated coastal management. Coastal Management 38: 317 335. Tomalin, T. 2005. Great grouper debate near end? St. Petersburg Times November 14, 2005: B 1. Trumbo, C. 1996. Constructing climate change: claims and frame s in US news coverage of an environmental issue. Public Understanding of Science 5: 269 283. Ungar, S. 2000. Knowledge, ignorance and the popular culture: climate change versus the ozone hole. Public Understanding of Science 9:297 312. U. S. Census Bureau. 2010. 2006 2010 American Community Survey. Retrieved 27 January 2012: http://factfinder2.census.gov Vaske, J. J., J. D. Absher, and A. D. Bright. 2007. Salient value similarity, social trust, and attitudes to ward wildland fire management strategies. Human Ecology Review 14: 223 232. Wagner, K. 2007. Mental models of flash floods and landslides. Risk Analysis 27:671 682. Weible, C. M. 2007. An advocacy coalition framework approach to stakeholder analysis: Unders tanding the political context of California marine protected area policy. Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory 17: 95 117.

PAGE 95

95 Whitmarsh, L. 2009 Public Understanding of Science 18: 401 420. Wilson, K. M. 1995. Mass media as sources of global warming knowledge. Mass Communications Review 22: 75 89. couga rs. Society and Animals 5: 95 116. Wolf, J. and S. C. Moser. 2011. Individual understandings, perceptions, and engagement with climate change: insights from in depth studies across the world. WIREs Climate Change 2:547 569. Wu, L. 2012. Exploring the new e environmental attitudes in China. The Journal of Environmental Education 43: 107 120. Zaksek, M. and J. L. Arvai. 2004. Toward improved communication about wildland fire: Mental models research to identify inf ormation needs for natural resource management. Risk Analysis 24:1503 1514. Zucker, H. G. 1978. The variable nature of news media influence. Communication Yearbook 2: 225 245.

PAGE 96

96 BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH J. Stuart Carlton was born and raised in New Orleans, Louis iana, where he graduated with a B.A. in English from Tulane University in 2001. He earned an M.S. in Fisheries Biology from the University of Georgia in 2004. After r eceiving his M.S., Stuart worked as a marine biologist for the Florida Fish and Wildlife Conservation Commission and a science teacher for Fitzgerald Middle School in Largo, Florida. He entered the Interdisciplinary Ecology Ph.D. program at the University of Florida in 200 7. Upon completion of his Ph.D., he will pursue a career in academics. Stuart has been married to his wife, Libby, since 2010, and they recently had a daughter, Harper, who has been rocess.