• TABLE OF CONTENTS
HIDE
 Title Page
 Acknowledgement
 Table of Contents
 List of Tables
 Abstract
 Introduction
 Statement of the problem
 Method
 Results
 Discussion
 Appendix: Cluster analysis
 Bibliography
 Biographical sketch














Title: Learning disabilities
CITATION PDF VIEWER THUMBNAILS PAGE IMAGE ZOOMABLE
Full Citation
STANDARD VIEW MARC VIEW
Permanent Link: http://ufdc.ufl.edu/UF00098645/00001
 Material Information
Title: Learning disabilities a multivariate search for subtypes
Physical Description: viii, 79 leaves : ; 28 cm.
Language: English
Creator: Darby, Roy Otto, 1945-
Publication Date: 1978
Copyright Date: 1978
 Subjects
Subject: Learning disabilities   ( lcsh )
Psychology thesis Ph. D   ( lcsh )
Dissertations, Academic -- Psychology -- UF   ( lcsh )
Genre: bibliography   ( marcgt )
non-fiction   ( marcgt )
 Notes
Thesis: Thesis--University of Florida.
Bibliography: Bibliography: leaves 74-78.
General Note: Typescript.
General Note: Vita.
Statement of Responsibility: by Roy Otto Darby III.
 Record Information
Bibliographic ID: UF00098645
Volume ID: VID00001
Source Institution: University of Florida
Holding Location: University of Florida
Rights Management: All rights reserved by the source institution and holding location.
Resource Identifier: alephbibnum - 000084892
oclc - 05296477
notis - AAK0238

Downloads

This item has the following downloads:

PDF ( 3 MBs ) ( PDF )


Table of Contents
    Title Page
        Page i
        Page ii
    Acknowledgement
        Page iii
    Table of Contents
        Page iv
    List of Tables
        Page v
    Abstract
        Page vi
        Page vii
        Page viii
    Introduction
        Page 1
        Page 2
        Page 3
        Page 4
        Page 5
        Page 6
        Page 7
        Page 8
        Page 9
        Page 10
        Page 11
        Page 12
        Page 13
        Page 14
        Page 15
        Page 16
        Page 17
        Page 18
        Page 19
        Page 20
        Page 21
    Statement of the problem
        Page 22
        Page 23
    Method
        Page 24
        Page 25
        Page 26
        Page 27
        Page 28
        Page 29
        Page 30
        Page 31
        Page 32
        Page 33
        Page 34
    Results
        Page 35
        Page 36
        Page 37
        Page 38
        Page 39
        Page 40
        Page 41
        Page 42
        Page 43
        Page 44
        Page 45
        Page 46
        Page 47
        Page 48
        Page 49
        Page 50
    Discussion
        Page 51
        Page 52
        Page 53
        Page 54
        Page 55
        Page 56
        Page 57
        Page 58
        Page 59
        Page 60
        Page 61
        Page 62
        Page 63
        Page 64
        Page 65
        Page 66
        Page 67
        Page 68
        Page 69
    Appendix: Cluster analysis
        Page 70
        Page 71
        Page 72
        Page 73
    Bibliography
        Page 74
        Page 75
        Page 76
        Page 77
        Page 78
    Biographical sketch
        Page 79
        Page 80
        Page 81
        Page 82
Full Text











LEARNING DISABILITIES: A MULTIVARIATE
SEARCH FOR SUBTYPES











By

ROY OTTO DARBY III


A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE COUNCIL OF
THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL
FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF
DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY





UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA

1978

































For Mary, with love















ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I would like to acknowledge with deepest gratitude the inspiration

and guidance of Dr. Paul Satz, who served as a superlative model of the

scientist-practitioner, without sacrificing the warmth and decency as a

human being that so characterize him.

To Dr. Jacquelin Goldman, whose high standards of professionalism

and fairness have always been coupled with her unflagging loyalty,

support and encouragement, I offer my respect and affection.

Special thanks are due to Dr. Roger K. Blashfield who served as

mentor and guide through the intricacies of the statistical procedures,

particularly cluster analysis, used in this project. Dr. Vernon

Van De Riet, Dr. Janet Larsen and Dr. Everette Hall have been patient,

receptive and encouraging in their support of this project while

serving as supervisory committee members.

My heartfelt thanks to Ms. Mary Ann Cruse, my comrade, for her

invaluable editorial assistance, and to Ms. Margi Tintner, my intrepid

typist.

In an endeavor of the psychological magnitude of a dissertation,

the commitment required is considerable. To those who have sacrificed

and suffered with me, my family, my love.















TABLE OF CONTENTS



Page

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ..... . . . . . . ..... iii

LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . ... . . . . v

ABSTRACT . . . . . . . . ... . . . . . vi

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . ... .. 1

Conceptual Issues . . . . . . . . . 2
Specific Developmental Dyslexia . . . . . . . 5
Classification Systems . . . . . . . . . 6

CHAPTER II STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM . . . . . .. 22

CHAPTER III METHOD .... . . . . . . .... 24

Subjects . . . . . . . . . . . . 24
Procedure . . . . . . . . . . . ... 25
Measures . . . . . . . . . . . . 31

CHAPTER IV RESULTS . . . . . . . . . . . 35

Phase I ..... .. . .... . . . . . . .35
Phase II . . . . . . . . ... .. ... . . 36
Phase III . . . . . . . . . .. . .. 42

CHAPTER V DISCUSSION . . . . . . . ..... 51

APPENDIX CLUSTER ANALYSIS . . . . . . . ... 70

BIBLIOGRAPHY . . . . . . . . . . . .. . 74

BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH . . . . . . . . .... 79















LIST OF TABLES

Table Page

1 Mean WRAT Discrepancy Scores for Achievement Subgroups . 29

2 Variable Correlations for Total Sample . ... . .. 30

3 Mean PPVT, WISC Similarities, Verbal Fluency, VMI and
Recognition-Discrimination Performance by Achievement
Subgroups . . . . . . . . . . . ... 39

4 Socioeconomic Status by Achievement Subgroups . . .. 43

5 Neurological Ratings by Achievement Sbugroups . . .. 44

6 Mean PPVT, WISC Similarities, Verbal Fluency, VMI and
Recognition-Discrimination Performance by Learning
Disabled Subtypes . . . . . . . . . 45

7 Neurological Ratings by Learning Disabled Subtypes . .50
















Abstract of Dissertation Presented to the Graduate Council
of the University of Florida in Partial Fulfillment
of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy


LEARNING DISABILITIES: A MULTIVARIATE
SEARCH FOR SUBTYPES

By

Roy Otto Darby III

December 1978

Chairman: Paul Satz
Major Department: Psychology

The purpose of this study was to conduct a systematic search for

subtypes within the learning disabled population. Cluster analytic

techniques were applied to the WRAT reading, spelling and arithmetic

scores of 236 boys with a mean age of 130 months. This relatively

unselected original sample included children at all levels of achieve-

ment. The initial analysis obtained 9 distinctive patterns of WRAT

scores. These subgroups were then compared through a multivariate

analysis of variance on measures of verbal fluency, WISC Similarities,

the Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration and a recognition-

discrimination task. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test estimates of

IQ, teacher ratings of socioeconomic status and neurological ratings

were also analyzed for subgroups.

As expected, there was a general trend for low-achievement groups

to perform more poorly on all variables. However, no subgroup obtained

a mean Peabody IQ of less than 90. One subgroup with average reading









but depressed arithmetic scores showed large decrements in perceptual-

motor performance. The differences between the two lowest achievement

subgroups were largely in degree of substandard performance. Both

these subgroups showed an extremely high proportion of children rated

"affected" on neurological examination. By virtue of their markedly

deficient WRAT achievement scores these subgroups were identified as

a learning disabled population.

For purposes of further analysis the low-achievement groups were

combined (N=89). A second cluster analysis utilizing the two language

and two perceptual-motor variables generated four unique cluster-

subtypes. No significant differences in WRAT achievement, socioeco-

nomic status or neurological ratings existed between subtypes. However,

differences in performance on the perceptual-motor and language tasks

were both statistically significant and heuristically meaningful.

The children of subtype 4 were clearly those with both impoverished

language and perceptual-motor skills. They were the only subtype to

obtain less than average Peabody IQ scores. Subtype 2 was characterized

by generally average scores except in verbal fluency where their per-

formance was significantly impaired. Subtype 3, on the other hand,

showed substantial deficits only on the perceptual-motor variables.

Subtype 1 constituted an enigma, as these children performed at levels

which equalled or exceeded the original sample mean (N=230) on every

variable. Comparisons on the 14 factor scores of the Children's Per-

sonality Questionnaire failed to support the hypothesis that motivation

problems or debilitating psychopathology were responsible for the poor

achievement of this subtype.









The results of this study point out the need for greater defini-

tional precision and specificity in the application of hypotheses in

research on the highly heterogeneous learning disabled population.

Finally, implications for future investigations are discussed.















CHAPTER I
INTRODUCTION

One of the fortuitous consequences of the growing human potential

and civil rights movements has been an increased awareness and sensi-

tivity to the effects of learning difficulties upon a child's devel-

opment. Learning disabilities now constitute a major educational and

social problem of the modern era. As incidence survey techniques and

measurement have improved, knowledge of the scope and depth of the

problem has increased. Some studies (e.g., Kline, 1972) have suggested

that at least 15 percent of all children in school suffer from severe

reading deficiencies. Additionally, preliminary results from longitu-

dinal research suggest the relationship of learning disability to a

surprising number of behavioral and emotional disorders, extending even

to the magnitude of schizophrenia (Robins, 1966). Studies linking read-

ing failure and adult criminal behavior (Wright, 1974) further under-

score the importance of the remediation of this disorder to the mainte-

nance of a productive society.

Although there can be little doubt that learning disabilities are

extremely common in almost every educational setting, there have been

certain difficulties in obtaining accurate estimates of the prevalence

of the disorder. Gaddes (1976) has pointed out that ambiguity results

from the application of imprecise definitional criteria to the variety

of learning disorders. Definitions and classifications may differ

dependent upon whether medical, psychological or educational models






2


are employed. Thus, while one of the few encouraging trends in the

field of learning disabilities has been the interest of a variety of

professional disciplines, another consequence of this interest has

been a proliferation of conceptual models and diagnostic labels.

This does not generate either a unitary body of knowledge about the

disorder, nor a comprehensive, coherent theory. Satz (1977) has

bluntly concluded that "the present state of affairs is such that

there can be no assurance that a diagnostic study will be accurate

nor that remedial instruction will be sufficient to meet a child's

needs" (p. 11).

Conceptual Issues

Applebee (1971) has identified two major deficiencies which have

contributed to the lack of success in research on reading retardation.

A major source of confusion has been an historical tendency to con-

struct restricted conceptual models which emphasize the role of var-

iables of interest to a single professional discipline (usually medical,

educational or psychological). A related problem has been a lack of

specificity and precision in defining the population identified as

reading disabled. Typically,the target population is defined through

the use of several exclusion criteria; selected variables on the re-

sulting group are then examined for unique patterns of deficits.

There has been an implicit, if erroneous, assumption of homogeneity

for these populations.

A second class of problems results when there is a lack of cor-

respondence between the statistical model used in analyzing the data

and the actual structure of the data. Applebee lists six models in

current usage. The simplest model proposes a single causal defect for









reading problems. Use of models of increasing complexity (e.g., mul-

tiple regression models) may reflect a growing awareness of the hetero-

geneity of the learning disabled population. In the sixth model, for

example, the assumption is made that there are several independent

syndromes, each dependent upon particular patterns of critical varia-

bles. Applebee speculates that different models may be appropriate

for different target populations.

Doehring (1976), citing the works of Applebee (1971) and Wiener

and Cromer (1967), has advanced the possibility that multiple processes

are involved in learning to read. He cautions that when a single-cause

model for learning disability is used, only a single syndrome may be

investigated, since the variability introduced by the presence of other

syndromes may confuse the interpretation of results. His review of

the literature reveals that the assumption of homogeneity for the

learning disabled population is probably unwarranted.

In his own studies Doehring compared children on measures related

to processing of letters, letter patterns, syllables, words and

syntactically-semantically related groups of words. Although the

study did not find distinguishing profiles for different groups of

reading disabled children, it did appear that the normal readers' per-

formance on the component skills measures conformed to a hierarchical

model, while the reading disabled children, in their "spotty" perform-

ance, seemed to fit more in accordance with an assembly model.

Doehring's multiple process position closely parallels that of Zig-

ler (1967a, 1967b, 1969) in the field of mental retardation. Zigler

has reviewed the conceptual bases of three distinct types of theories

of mental retardation (difference, defect and developmental models).









A difference theory states that a clinical population has more (or

less) of a certain attribute. A defect theory states that the popu-

lation lacks the attribute. Finally, a developmental theory proposes

that the discrepancies between normal and pathological populations

are related only to the rate of acquisition and the limit of achieve-

ment that each may approach. Zigler concludes that the mentally

retarded population is not homogeneous, even though it may be defined

by a single criterion (e.g., an IQ less than 70). He proposes that

an initial distinction at least be made between the organic subgroup

of retardates and the developmental-familial group.

Wiener and Cromer (1967) applying similar concepts to the reading

retarded population have identified four assumptions which determine

how one conceptualizes reading disability. If one assumes a defect

as the cause of the disorder, a neurological explanation is most easily

suggested. An assumption of deficiency draws attention to such areas

as phonetic skills or general language ability. A disruption model

lends itself to consideration of emotional factors. Finally, a differ-

ence model emphasizes the mismatch between the behavior patterns of the

individual and those required by his environment. These assumptions

lead to investigation of three kinds of factors: sensory-perceptual

(physiological), experiential-learning (educational), and personality-

emotional (psychological). Through the relationships of the factors

with each other, different types of disabled readers may be produced.

Benton (1975), after a thorough review of current evidence, con-

cludes that such evidence is too contradictory and inconsistent to

support the assumption that all dyslexic children suffer the same basic

deficiency. Benton cautions that, in order to make meaningful statements









about reading failure, it is necessary to specify carefully the type

of behavior which constitutes the failure, as well as the level of

the failure. Silverberg and Silverberg (1977), for example, have

demonstrated that different reading tests may produce different

estimates of a child's reading level.

Benton next proposes locating the cognitive and functional corre-

lates associated with the faulty reading performance. Ultimately,

the search for the neurological-genetic substrate which subsumes these

correlates may then be conducted. Benton proposes that a classifica-

tion system at least distinguish between poor readers with no accom-

panying problems and those with concomitant deficiencies in other

skill areas. This distinction would then be a first step toward the

definition of more homogeneous classes of disabled learners.

Specific Developmental Dyslexia

While the evidence has persistently pointed to multiple determin-

ants of reading disability, investigators have most often concentrated

their attention on a population identified by the World Federation of

Neurology as exhibiting specific developmental dyslexia. The disorder

has been defined as one ". manifested by difficulty in learning to

read despite conventional instruction, adequate intelligence and socio-

cultural opportunity. It is dependent upon fundamental cognitive dis-

abilities which are frequently of constitutional origin" (Waites, 1968,

p. 16). Various labels have been applied to this hypothetical disorder

including specific reading disability, strephosymbolia, congenital

word blindness, reading retardation and unexpected reading failure.

The World Federation definition hints that the disorder is a unitary

one, yet membership in the classification is based upon exclusion









criteria which imprecisely delimit it. A vague unproven etiology is

also implied, but no clue to possible mechanisms involved is given.

Under such circumstances the homogeneity of the group certainly becomes

suspect.

A preliminary study (Taylor, Satz and Friel, 1977) was recently

completed which attempted to determine whether there was any clinical

utility to the diagnosis of specific developmental dyslexia. Taking

particular pains to satisfy definitional requirements, they sought to

compare "dyslexic" with "non-dyslexic" disabled readers, and both groups

with normal readers. The measures selected for the comparisons in-

cluded: (1) severity of reading failure, (2) parental reading and

spelling competency, (3) neurological rating, (4) math skills, (5) neuro-

psychological performance, (6) reversal errors in reading and (7) per-

sonality traits. The tasks uniformly failed to differentiate between

the two disabled groups, but differentiated the total group of disabled

readers from the group of normal readers. The results challenge the

traditional notion of dyslexia as being easily dissociated from other

reading problems. The investigators, while not rejecting the concept

of the existence of specific subgroups of disabled readers, stress the

need to operationalize definitions and trim surplus meaning from con-

cepts in the area. They call for further studies to discover clinically

useful groupings, perhaps based on patterns of deficits, neuropsycho-

logical performance, the kinds of errors in reading and spelling, and

even according to prognosis.

Classification Systems

Historically the efforts of most researchers have been directed

toward the isolation of the "essential nature" of what has been









regarded as a homogeneous disorder. It is interesting, therefore, to

note in retrospect that Monroe (1932) performed a study well ahead of

its time in its suggestion of the involvement of multiple determinants

of learning disability. This well-designed study tentatively identified

subgroups based on the source and nature of the referral. One group

of children was referred for assorted problems in development and

behavior. Another group was identified by teachers and parents as

having specific difficulties in reading. The final group was composed

of children with borderline or defective intelligence. Monroe ob-

tained a reading index based on the comparison of a composite reading

grade with chronological age, mental age and an arithmetic score. She

then classified the errors made in reading into ten types. It was

found that patterns of types of errors did emerge, but that different

"causes" could produce the same profile. Monroe discussed the various

defects which could give rise to reading disability, the pattern of

errors thay may result and the remediation methods recommended for

children with each type of error pattern.

Robinson (1946) similarly utilized the skills of a team of spe-

cialists representing the different professions to investigate the

proposition of multiple determinants in learning disabilities. The

causal factors he identified are similar to those of Monroe (1932).

Both studies emphasize that visual maladjustments are commonly asso-

ciated with reading problems. For example, Robinson's close scrutiny

of 30 reading disabled children showed 73 percent to have visual diffi-

culties, although the visual handicap was judged as causal in only

one-third of these cases. Additional causes identified were neurologi-

cal compromise, auditory and speech difficulties, physical deficiencies,









intellectual deficiency, emotional and personality deviancy, social

and environmental handicaps, endocrine abnormalities and perhaps, prob-

lems of cerebral dominance. The foresight of these studies is quite

remarkable and undoubtedly they have not received the attention they

deserve.

Blom and Jones (1970) have sought to divide classification systems

into four major types. One group of systems focuses on "descriptive

reading behaviors" (a useful approach for teachers and educators since

one or more of the symptoms can usually be observed in students).

Distinctions may, for example, be made between children who show diffi-

culties in oral reading and those with poor silent reading. Other

descriptive-behavior systems might differentiate children according to

the sensory modality impaired (visual, auditory, etc.).

A second group of systems is based on etiology. One of the sim-

plest is that of Eisenberg (1966), who dichotomizes factors in reading

disability into sociopsychological sources (for example, defects in

teaching or deficiencies in motivation), and psychophysiological sources

(defects in intellect, sensory processing, brain functioning or general

ability).

Blom and Jones next point out that most theoretical systems

develop classifications consistent with their theories. This third

type includes, for example, models based upon psychoanalytic ego theory,

upon statistical constructs and upon psycholinguistic theory.

Finally, some systems attempt to exhaust all possibilities and

compile a complete nosological system (e.g., that of Blom and Jones,

1970).









Many researchers have hypothesized that the two major origins of

reading difficulties are neurological defect and genetic predisposition.

A rudimentary classification system may be drawn if this dichotomy is

accepted. Rugel and Mitchell (1977) predicted that one group of read-

ing disabled children would show symptoms of minimal brain dysfunction

including hyperactivity, distractibility and perceptual-motor problems.

It was anticipated that the children of the other group would show

difficulties in auditory sequential-memory, auditory discrimination

and visual-spatial perception, factors felt to be inherited. A twenty

item behavioral rating scale, a test of auditory vigilance and measures

of skin conductance were employed as dependent measures. The data

seemed to show some support for their hypothesis, but the authors

speculated that there may be a need for two categories of the familial

poor readers those with MBD symptoms and those without.

Silver (1971) notes that the distinction between learning dis-

ability and minimal brain dysfunctions is often poorly drawn. In a

study of 556 children he attempted to identify familial patterns among

children with neurologically based learning disability. On the basis

of his data he concludes that it is necessary to specify both the par-

ticular type of learning disability (e.g., perceptual-motor, memory,

motor, etc.) as well as the etiology. Silver's data showed that in

his neurological population familial patterns were present in 30 to 40

percent of the cases.

One of the more common classification systems in both formal and

informal usage distinguishes between children whose learning problems

are but one manifestation of a more general impairment and those who

show more circumscribed learning disturbance. The distinction between









secondary and primary reading retardation advanced by Rabinovitch and

associates (Rabinovitch, Drew, DeJong, Ingram and Whithey, 1954;

Rabinovitch, 1968) represents an early approach for specifying sub-

groups of reading disabled children. Conceptually the primary reading

retardation group fits the criteria for the definition of specific

developmental dyslexia. Secondary reading retardation implies that

the disorder is the result of other pathology or condition enceph-

alopathy, emotional disturbance, poor language experience or motivational/

opportunity factors. This group of children was shown to have a

better prognosis than the primary reading retardation group who were

hypothesized to suffer from a basic disturbed pattern of neurological

organization.

Ingram, Mason and Blackburn (1970) examined 82 children selected

for a two-year discrepancy between mental age (as measured by the

Stanford-Binet) and reading age. Elaborate pre-, peri- and postnatal

histories were obtained along with detailed medical and neurological

examinations. Data on reading, personality, perceptual-motor skills

and speech were also collected. While an earlier study by Ingram (1966)

had found evidence for a three-way classification, the data on these

82 children suggested two subgroups: those with a general disorder

and those with a specific learning disturbance. The two groups did

not differ significantly in severity of reading failure nor in incidence

of family history of reading difficulties. Examination of the "general"

group, however, revealed a greater frequency of abnormal births and

developmental histories, as well as positive findings on neurological

examinations and abnormal EEG's. The "specific" group showed a higher

percentage of audiophonic difficulties and tended to make more primitive









types of errors in reading. The authors concluded that severe reading

difficulty can be present without brain abnormality and, significantly,

is likely to be part of more general educational problems.

Keeney (1968) also accepted the primary-secondary classification

but argued that, even with limited etiological knowledge, an attempt

should be made to form a more comprehensive classification system.

He proposed three additional major divisions. His category "slow

readers" is employed to distinguish those children whose reading diffi-

culties stem from visual handicaps, auditory impairments or hypothy-

roid states. "Acquired dyslexia" covers the somewhat rarer cases

where there are lesions of the dominant hemisphere, angular gyrus or

splenium. Finally, he proposed a "mixed" category to account for

children with positive profiles fitting two or more of the other

classifications.

Yule and Rutter (1976) have used more powerful techniques for

differentiating "general reading backwardness" from "specific reading

retardation." Responding to the need to operationalize definitions

and concepts used with reading handicapped children, these researchers

defined "unexpected reading failure" as a discrepancy of 2-years 4-

months or more between reading level and an expectancy score derived

from chronological age and assessed intellectual level. Their "general"

group was defined simply on the basis of performance 2-years 4-months

below chronological age, irrespective of intellectual level. These

groups, of course, had many members in common. Additional power for

this study was provided by the examination of an entire population of

children. The "specific" group obtained higher intelligence scores,

was more likely to be male, typically showed delays in development of









speech and language and tended to be more refractory to attempts at

intervention. The "general" group had a higher incidence of manifest

neurological conditions, motor and praxic difficulties, and left-right

confusion. These latter children tended more frequently to come from

lower class families and to show generally greater academic improvement

in all subjects except arithmetic.

Significantly, no core group fitting the definition of develop-

mental dyslexia was able to be isolated in this study. By contrast

the concept of "specific reading retardation" can theoretically be

applied to children at any level of intellectual ability as long as

reading ability is not commensurate with IQ. This concept does not

carry any implication for etiology, whether genetically or socially

transmitted. Yule and Rutter's concept of specific reading retardation

does not correspond to the unitary syndrome idea often associated with

the term dyslexia. Rather, specific reading retardation is viewed as

the result of the complex interaction of multiple factors.

While some investigators (e.g., Taylor, Satz and Friel, 1977) have

questioned the existence of a distinct dyslexic syndrome, others have

sought to identify, within the dyslexic population, subgroups defined

by different patterns of deficits. Mattis, French and Rapin (1975)

have reviewed the literature concerned with neuropsychological param-

eters of learning disability. They found that previous investigations

have related the presence of learning disability to (1) the development

of perceptual stability and the ability to transfer information between

sensory modalities, (2) the development of language and speech fluency,

(3) the acquisition of gross and fine motor coordination and (4) the

development of lateral awareness and dominance. With these guidelines,









Mattis et al. compared the performance of brain-damaged children with

normal reading, on a variety of neuropsychological measures. It was

found that these measures did not discriminate groups of dyslexics

(brain-damaged versus non-brain-damaged). However, on the basis of

inspection, three patterns of deficits seemed to emerge, accounting

for about 90 percent of the poor readers.

The language disorder was characterized by verbal retrieval prob-

lems anomiaa), intact visuo-constructional skills, and lowered vocabu-

lary scores. These children often produced lower WISC Verbal IQ's than

Performance IQ's, although this relationship was not diagnostic in

and of itself. The investigators noted that children who show anomia

after age 8 seem to be at exceptionally high risk for learning disabil-

ity.

The motor speech disorder (labelled the articulation and dysco-

ordination syndrome) was defined by specific deficits in the production

of sounds and words. The dyspraxia present in these children was more

likely to be of the buccal-lingual than of the dental-palatal type.

Verbal and Performance IQ's for the group were more nearly equal.

Children classified as suffering from a visual-perceptual disorder

appeared to have failed to establish a stable and reliable association

between sounds and letters. Notably, this disorder was not manifested

on constructional tasks, although on tasks of visual integration and

complex perception significant impairment was observed.

On the basis of this evidence the principal processes critical to

reading were concluded to involve the adequate development of language

symbolization, intact visual-spatial perception and the ability to

produce fluent speech. The distribution of the three syndromes was









different for the brain-damaged and non-brain-damaged dyslexics. A

language disorder was most common in the brain-damaged dyslexics, fol-

lowed by a motor-speech disorder and to a significantly lesser extent

a visual-perceptual disorder. In the non-brain-damaged dyslexics the

motor-speech disorder and the language disorder were reversed in fre-

quency.

Denckla (1972) has identified three clinical syndromes (a specific

language disturbance, a specific visuo-spatial disability and a dys-

control syndrome) which roughly correspond to those of Mattis et al.

(1975). Denckla, however, found that approximately 70 percent of dis-

abled learners either produced mixed deficits or did not fit into any

of the three categories. Denckla's dyscontrol syndrome is further

distinguished by the presence of both poor muscular coordination and

increased levels of activity (hyperkinesis).

Numerous other studies have suggested the existence of subgroups

with either primary language deficits or primary visuo-spatial deficits.

Cole and Kraft (1964) divided their 36 subjects into five groups which

included these, along with (1) dyslexics without general language or

visuo-spatial defect, (2) dyslexics with mixed defects and (3) specific

learning disability without dyslexia. This latter group was quite

heterogeneous and contained children with apraxic difficulties, specific

spelling disability, with dysgraphia but without apraxia,and with

abnormal speech development. Although the reliability of this categori-

zation is questionable owing to the small number of subjects in each

group (e.g., the visuo-spatial group contained only 4 members) and the

methods used to classify the children, it represents an attempt to

recognize additional patterns of disability among these children.









Among their subjects the language disorder was easily the most common,

followed by the mixed group. Cole and Kraft found a high incidence of

both abnormal neurological examinations (86 percent) and family history

for learning problems (50 percent) among all of the groups, a finding

common in many studies.

A comprehensive study of learning disability in children and their

siblings was undertaken by Owen et al. (1971). The broad objectives

of the project were to identify the characteristics of different learn-

ing disabilities and to attempt to isolate causal and familial patterns.

Using an inspection technique five groups of the educationally handi-

capped population were preselected and they and their siblings were

compared with matched controls and their siblings. Subjects were com-

pared on 87 variables. Unfortunately the classification system was

based on criteria which yielded overlapping, confabulated categories.

Three of the five groups were based on WISC performance patterns (high

Full Scale IQ, low Full Scale IQ and relatively higher Performance than

Verbal IQ). The fourth category reflected etiological criteria (high

incidence of medical and neurological abnormality) and the final cate-

gory was based upon behavioral dimensions (social deviancy). Less than

half of the children could be placed in a single category. The familial,

neurological, intellectual and performance characteristics reported for

each of the groups must be regarded as highly tentative because of these

difficulties. The higher-Performance IQ group, for example, showed

great similarity to the medical-neurological group along a number of

performance dimensions, although the former group was in other ways the

purest.









The study produced more significant findings regarding the charac-

teristics of their dyslexic group as a whole. In their group there

were very few gross neurological abnormalities noted on either physical

examination or EEG. However, these children were judged to be more

neurologically immature and showed greater deficits on neuropsychologi-

cal tasks including right-left discrimination, auditory tapping and

simultaneous tactile perception. Although these dyslexic children

performed more poorly on psychomotor tasks,they were as capable of per-

ceiving their errors as the control group. Both parents of these

children were found to have performed less well in high school English.

While the dyslexic children's fathers obtained lower overall WRAT

scores than fathers of the control group, the dyslexic group's mothers

had obtained poorer math grades in high school than mothers of the

control group.

Finally, the study addressed the often-raised issue of whether

reliable WISC subtest score relationships exist which distinguish chil-

dren with learning disabilities. The data suggest that dyslexic chil-

dren tend to produce relatively depressed scores on the Arithmetic,

Digit Span and Coding subtests and relatively elevated scores on Picture

Completion. The disabled children were found to have a greater inci-

dence of higher WISC Performance IQ's than Verbal IQ's. In addition,

reading and spelling scores (WRAT) were found to be less highly corre-

lated for this group.

In his review of the evidence regarding WISC subtest patterns,

Huelsman (1970) found that there was general but weak support for the

hypothesis that disabled readers tended to show lower Verbal IQ's than

Performance IQ's. There appeared to be a strong need to identify









operational subtypes before making generalizations regarding WISC sub-

test patterns. Huelsman's own subjects obtained a "believable differ-

ence" between PIQ and VIQ in only 20 percent of the cases. It was

found that no patterns of WISC subtest scores (including the often-

reported lowered Arithmetic, Coding and Information scores) reliably

identified reading disabled children.

A different approach to subtyping is exemplified in the works of

DeHirsch and Jansky (1968), Boder (1968, 1970, 1971) and Doehring and

Hoshko (1976). The defining characteristics in these classification

systems derive from single or multiple performance measures as, for

example, achievement test scores, school grades or reading skills

scores.

DeHirsch and Jansky (1968) retrospectively examined the protocols

of kindergarten children and followed their reading progress. They scru-

tinized standardized test scores and developmental histories, as well

as measures of language and perceptual-motor ability. On the basis of

reading performance the children were divided into high achievers, slow

starters and failing readers. The reading failure children in this

study were observed to be more immature, have diffuse deficits in oral

language, unstable auditory and visual perception and inferior perceptual-

motor skills. Hyperactivity was observed to occur with greater fre-

quency in this group. At the year's end these children continued to

show widespread deficits in contrast to the slow starters, who had

largely made up their initial failure.

Boder (1968, 1970, 1971) has long argued that it is of importance

to know how a child reads as well as at what level. She noted that some

children exhibited difficulties in associating sounds with appropriate









symbols. In her earlier work (1968) these children were called "visile"

while a second group with visualizing difficulties were identified as

audilee." A third group was included for those with mixed deficits.

In her later works Boder (1970, 1971) integrated these conceptual

divisions with actual test performance on diagnostic reading and spelling

tasks. Those children who demonstrated adequate sight vocabulary but

impaired word attack skills and poor phonics (the visile children) were

labeled as "dysphonetic." Other children were successful in approximating

unknown words through phonetic sounding, but were deficient in recogniz-

ing words that should have been in their sight vocabulary. These chil-

dren formed the "dyseidetic" group. (In the classroom such children

tend to read very laboriously but possess near normal word attack skills.)

The remaining group who demonstrated both patterns of deficits and

overall poor reading were classified as "alexic." Camp and Dolcourt

(1977) linked this latter group to those children identified in other

systems as having generalized learning difficulties or non-specific

reading problems.

Doehring and Hoshko (1976) utilized more advanced statistical methods

in classifying children with reading handicaps according to their per-

formance on 31 tests of reading-related skills. These tests required

rapid matching responses to simple sets of letters, syllables, words and

sentences. The method used to group the children is a variation of

factor analytic procedure known as "Q-technique." The "Q-technique" is

an "inverted" method which groups together individuals who show similar

patterns of test scores. A factor is defined in this technique by the

performance of individuals who have high loadings on that factor. In

this case subgroups of children were defined on the basis of reading









performance, and the profile of each subgroup was examined. Two samples

were drawn. The first was composed of children whose primary difficul-

ties were in reading, while the second sample included children with

more general learning disorders, language disorders and mental retarda-

tion. The number of subjects in each sample (34 and 31 respectively)

was uncomfortably small for analyses of this sort.

Three principal factors emerged for the first and second samples

and four for the combined samples. In both samples three major sub-

groups were found. In the reading retarded sample the first subgroup

of children performed well on visual and auditory-visual matching but

poorly on oral reading tests involving words and syllables. The second

subgroup was characterized by good visual scanning but very poor

auditory-visual matching and poor oral reading. The last subgroup showed

good visual and auditory-visual matching of single letters (but not of

words and syllables) and poor oral word, sentence and syllable reading

skills.

The first two subgroups of the second sample resembled those of

the first sample. The remaining subgroup was characterized by slow

visual matching. The authors attempted to relate their statistically

derived groups to those proposed by previous investigators none of

whom used multivariate correlational procedures. Their results are

difficult to interpret in light of the complex patterns of the con-

stituent variables obtained by the analyses. However, it was concluded

that the use of statistical classification techniques could greatly

facilitate the achievement of a consensus regarding the number and the

types of developmental reading disabilities.








A recent study by Rourke and Finlayson (1978) examined patterns of

academic performance in relationship to neuropsychological variables.

An earlier study (Rourke, Young and Flewelling, 1971) demonstrated that

differential patterns of WRAT subtest performance could be related to

particular patterns of WISC Verbal IQ-Performance IQ discrepancies.

The earlier study found that performance on the WRAT Arithmetic subtest

was more dependent upon visual-spatial skills than upon abilities of a

verbal nature. The current project sought to determine whether children

who exhibited varying patterns of academic abilities would also exhibit

unique, meaningful and consistent patterns of visuo-spatial and verbal

behaviors.

Children were placed into three groups. One of the possible weak-

nesses of the study was the composition of these groups. Group 1 was

labeled as deficient in reading, spelling and arithmetic when WRAT per-

formance on all tasks was at least 2 years below expected grade level

placement. Group 2 was selected on the basis of their WRAT spelling and

reading scores falling at least 1.8 years below their WRAT arithmetic

scores. It should be noted, however, that this arithmetic performance

still represented a deficit of some magnitude (mean grade level=4.86)

in relation to the mean age for the group (143.67 months). The mean

arithmetic score for Group 2 was significantly higher than for Group 1.

However, when Group 3 was selected, arithmetic scores similar to those

of Group 2 were then considered deficits, and were, in fact, the basis

for identification of this group. Group 3's reading and spelling scores

were significantly better than either Group 1 or 2, who did not differ

significantly on these tests. Given these relationships between groups,

the identity of each group is somewhat obscured by the confabulation









of absolute criteria (as, for example, in the selection of a two-year

deficit cut-off) with criteria based on relative performance (as, for

example, used in the definition of Group 2). In their defense the

authors argue that their results suggest patterns of performance rather

than levels of performance are the more critical dimension.

Differential hypotheses were advanced concerning the expected per-

formance on visuo-spatial, visuo-perceptual, auditory-perceptual and

verbal measures. On nine of the 10 verbal and auditory-perceptual meas-

ures, the performance of Group 3 was found to be significantly superior

to that of Groups 1 and 2. Groups 1 and 2 did not differ from each

other on any of the variables, but were superior to Group 3 on the

visuo-perceptual and visuo-spatial measures. Additionally, Groups 1 and

2 showed significantly lower Verbal IQ's and higher Performance IQ's

compared to Group 3, although the Full Scale IQ's for the three groups

did not differ.

The authors conclude that their results are consistent with the

view that deficiencies in arithmetic are due to difficulties in visual-

spatial organization and integration, abilities believed to be dependent

upon the integrity of the right cerebral hemisphere. Similarly, Groups 1

and 2 appeared to perform in a fashion similar to that expected were

they to have a relatively dysfunctional left cerebral hemisphere.

The authors speculate that differences between Groups 1 and 2 may

not lie along the particular dimensions examined in their study. They

call for further studies to compare the motor, psychomotor and tactile-

kinesthetic performance of their subgroups in order to investigate

possible alternative dimensions, as well as to provide further data on

the role of the right and left cerebral hemispheres in academic perform-

ance.















CHAPTER II
STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM

The preceding review suggests that most investigators working in

the field of learning disabilities recognize the heterogeneity of that

group of children whose academic performance falls below that of their

age peers. Various schema have been devised attempting to account for

the considerable variance exhibited by these children on almost all

variables. The assumption is often made that, through the discrimin-

ation of subgroups within the general learning disabled population,

greater precision in the identification of causal factors would be ob-

tained. Ultimately, the application of differential treatment methods

could take place. More accurate prognostic statements could then be

made for children who exhibit particular types of learning difficulties.

In light of the potential importance of identifying subtypes within

the learning disabled population, there is a surprising lack of systema-

tic investigation in this area.

In current usage the term "dyslexic" is often applied to any child

who is behind in reading. Thus, there is little agreement regarding

prevalence or epidemiology, much less the underlying mechanisms involved.

Some researchers have attempted to conform to the World Federation of

Neurology's definition of "specific developmental dyslexia" (Waites,

1968) on the assumption that a homogeneous sample is being selected.

Recent studies (e.g., Taylor, Satz and Friel, 1977) fail to support

this assumption, and the empirical utility of the concept appears to be

suspect.









Many studies that have addressed the subtype problem as a primary

issue (rather than as a "nuisance" parameter) have imposed a priori

schema upon the data, sympathetic to a particular theoretical position.

Thus, children have been sorted according to such criteria as neuro-

logically impaired versus familial history of learning disability, or

according to a certain pre-determined pattern of test performance.

The validity of such classification schemes is too often treated as

assumption rather than as hypothesis. Frequently, the method of

assigning children to these categories is based solely upon inspection

of quite complex data sets, rather than upon more rigorous systematic

statistical methods. The study by Doehring and Hoshko (1977) stands

as a notable exception to the "inspection" approach. Unfortunately, the

precision of their sorting technique failed to reduce the complex pat-

terns of performance to readily interpretable dimensions.

The need for systematic, rigorous studies to delineate subtypes

is dictated both by the practical and theoretical implications of such

discoveries, and by the present rather dismal lack of success (or at

least agreement) in this area.















CHAPTER III
METHOD

Subjects

The subjects for the study were those children who participated in

the longitudinal study of Satz and associates (1973, 1974, 1977) who

were administered the Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT) in year 6

(grade 5) of their schooling. The children in the original standardiza-

tion sample of the Satz and Friel (1973) study consisted of 497 white

male kindergarten pupils in the Alachua County, Florida, public school

system and the University of Florida Laboratory School. This number

represented 96 percent of that population enrolled in 20 county schools.

A second group of children, identified as the cross-validation sample

(N=181) was initially tested in 1971. The total sample for the present

study (N=236) was drawn from these two groups. The mean age of these

children at time of administration of the WRAT was 130.0 months (SD=3.9)

with a range of 124 to 143 months.

The Satz studies utilized white males exculsively, in an attempt to

provide a more homogeneous sample of children who would be at higher

risk for developmental dyslexia (boys), and who would be less likely to

be culturally disadvantaged (whites). The large sample size was felt to

insure that subgroups of failing readers could be identified and studied

in subsequent years.

Major follow-up examinations of the children were conducted in

years 3 and 6 of the study. Teacher ratings and standardized achieve-

ment tests (administered through the school system) were obtained in









intervening years. For the present study data from these previous

examinations were available.

Procedure
In the initial phase of the present study the relatively unselected

sample of 236 children was sorted into naturally occurring subgroups

according to achievement test scores. These subgroups were next sur-

veyed to determine if there were patterns of performance on intellectual,

language and perceptual-motor tasks which further distinguished these

groups. Attention was then directed towards the central question of

the study, the identification of subtypes within the population of

learning disabled children.

The first step in the process consisted of the application of

cluster analytic techniques to Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT)

reading, spelling and arithmetic scores. These scores were entered in

the form of discrepancy scores which were derived by comparing a child's

chronological age with the age-equivalent score obtained on each sub-

test. Thus, a discrepancy score of "-24" in reading would indicate

that the performance was 24 months behind that expected on the basis

of the child's chronological age.

Cluster analysis is a set of procedures whose most common use is

to form a classification system from a data set. This is accomplished

by grouping together individuals most similar to each other on the

component cluster variables.

Phase I

All clustering procedures used in this study were contained in the

CLUSTAN 1C program (Wishart, 1975). For the WRAT data a hierarchical,

agglomerative, average-linkage method employing a squared euclidean









distance similarity coefficient was selected. The average-linkage

method combined with the euclidean distance measure is more likely to

permit clusters to emerge which do not fit the general trend of the

data. It was known that WRAT reading, spelling and arithmetic scores

are highly correlated for the general population (Jastak and Jastak,

1976). Therefore, it was anticipated that there would be a strong

tendency to form clusters composed of reading, spelling and arithmetic

scores at the same level (e.g., high, low or average). The structure

of the data would then be largely that of a linear scale from lowest to

highest achievement. Some other methods and similarity coefficients

(e.g., those which minimize an error sum of squares) would be more

likely to form spherical clusters and to obscure any deviations from

the major trends of the data.

After the completion of the initial clustering, the composition

of these clusters was then re-examined using Procedure Relocate (Wis-

hart, 1975) in order to generate a local optimum solution. The rationale

behind this latter procedure as well as the method used to determine

the optimum number of clusters present in the data is discussed in the

Appendix.

Phase II

Phase I established a preliminary classification system. To con-

firm the validity of the cluster solution a multivariate analysis of

variance (MANOVA) was applied to the WRAT reading, spelling and

arithmetic scores to test for the effects of subgroups. Subgroups

were then compared again utilizing MANOVA, on two language measures,

WISC Similarities scaled scores (Wechsler, 1949) and Verbal Fluency

(Satz and Friel, 1973). They were additionally compared on a perceptual









task, Recognition-Discrimination (Small, 1968), and a perceptual-motor

developmental index, the Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration

(Beery, 1967). Utilizing chi-square tests for independence additional

comparisons were made on a teacher's rating of socioeconomic status

(low versus average or above) and a rating of neurological status

(normal, equivocal or affected) rendered by qualified physicians.

Finally, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (Dunn, 1965) IQ's for the sub-

groups were subjected to an analysis of variance. These measures were

selected in an attempt to determine if the groupings were associated

with distinctive patterns of development, background, constitution or

abilities and, additionally provided an external check on the classifi-

cation system.

All multivariate and univariate analyses were conducted using the

General Linear Models (GLM) procedure of the Statistical Analysis

Systems (SAS) program (Barr, Goodnight, Sall and Helwig, 1976). The

MANOVA procedure is the multivariate analogue of the univariate analysis

of variance and was considered appropriate to the present data in order

to decrease the probability of a Type I error which might result from

the repetition of individual univariate analyses for each of the mul-

tiple dependent variables. When significant effects were found for

subgroup in the MANOVA, individual variables were subjected to uni-

variate analyses. Individual means were compared using post hoc Dun-

can's Multi-Range Tests (Winer, 1971).

Phase III

While the first two parts of the study surveyed a broad range of

achievement levels, this phase of the study concentrated on those chil-

dren whose performance was substantially lower than that of their age









peers. The two lowest scoring subgroups (N=89) were combined into a

single group to be reanalyzed. The reading, spelling and arithmetic

scores for these groups, as indicated in Table 1, are sufficiently

depressed to suggest that these children suffer significant difficul-

ties in learning.

The combined group of low achievers was then reclustered on the

basis of WISC Similarities, Verbal Fluency, Developmental Test of

Visual-Motor Integration and Recognition-Discrimination scores in an

attempt to identify subtypes within the learning disabled population.

Again a hierarchical, agglomerative method was applied. However, be-

cause these variables were not as highly correlated as the reading,

spelling and arithmetic scores of Phase I (Table 2), the method which

assigns individuals to clusters in a manner which minimizes the error

sum of squares (minimum variance method) was now appropriate for use.

As in Phase I a local optimum solution was then sought through Pro-

cedure Relocate.

The resulting clusters were tentatively identified as subtypes of

the learning disabled population. A MANOVA search for differences be-

tween subtypes on WRAT reading, spelling and arithmetic scores was

made. In order to establish the statistical validity of the subtypes,

the clusters were then examined through a multivariate analysis of

variance of the clustered variables. Individual analyses of variance

followed by post hoc tests (Duncan's Multi-Range Tests) were applied as

in Phase II. Similarly, chi-square statistical tests for independence

were computed for the socioeconomic status and neurological measures.

Finally, an analysis of variance of Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test

scores for the groups was conducted.












TABLE 1


Mean WRAT Discrepancy Scores For Achievement Subgroups


SUBGROUP NUMBER

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

.9

10

11

12

Total Sample


READING*

42.8 A

40.7 A

25.2 B

10.8 C

2.5 D

1.0 D

- 5.7 E

-20.5 F

-31.2 G

56.6

66.0

74.0

.6


* Means followed by the same
different within variables
Alpha level = .05).


SPELLING* ARITHMETIC*


24.9 A

5.9 B

4.3 B

- .2 C

-14.9 E

- 8.5 D

-16.5 E

-26.7 F

-34.7 G

61.7

86.0

67.5

-11.2


11.8 A

- 9.6 D

2.6 C

-12.7 E

-23.2 G

6.4 B

- 8.6 D

-16.7 F

-27.4 H

-10.0

8.0

26.0

-11.0


letter are not significantly
(Duncan's Multi-Range Test,











TABLE 2


Variable Correlations for Total Sample


WRAT WRAT WRAT
READING SPELLING ARITHMETIC


WRAT SPELLING .87


WRAT ARITHMETIC .65


PPVT .56


SIM .60


VF .49


VMI .48


RD .37


Note: All correlations (Pearson


.66


.47 .34


.54 .47 .63


.49 .40 .46


.47 .48 .32


.34 .33 .44


Product-Moment Correlations) are significant


VF VMI


.44


.43


.35


at p .001.










Measures

Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT)

This test was first standardized in 1936 (Jastak and Jastak, 1976).

Since that time it has gained widespread acceptance as an economical

and reasonably accurate estimate of a child's level of school achieve-

ment in reading, spelling and arithmetic. Criticisms of the instrument

have noted that the subtests tap only a limited range of behaviors.

For example, the reading subtest is a measure based solely on word

recognition. However, studies by Rourke and Orr (1977) suggest that

the WRAT is as powerful a discriminator of normal and disabled readers

as are the Reading, Word Knowledge and Word Discrimination subtests of

the Metropolitan Achievement Test. Validation studies cited by Jastak

and Jastak (1976) generally point to moderately high correlations with

the Stanford Achievement Test, the Woody-Sangren Silent Reading Test as

well as other frequently used reading assessment instruments.

Language and Perceptual Motor Measures

These measures were selected for their presumed ability to measure

performance in the important areas of cognitive-language and perceptual-

motor development. In the longitudinal studies of Satz and associates

(1973, 1974, 1977) WISC Similarities and Verbal Fluency were found to

load highly on a factor identified as verbal-conceptual ability. These

measures contributed heavily to the discriminative power of this factor

to identify children at high and low risk for learning problems, in the

age range examined in the present study.

Among the tests of non-verbal abilities administered in the Satz

predictive battery, the Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration

and Recognition-Discrimination showed the highest loadings over time on








the factor labeled sensori-perceptual-motor. As of year 6 of the

study they demonstrated their ability to predict future reading success

from year 1 (predictive validity), as well as their ability to discrim-

inate between groups at year 6 (concurrent validity). However, this

factor reportedly had less power at year 6 than the verbal-conceptual

factor (Satz et al., 1977).

WISC Similarities (SIM). The composition, scoring and validity of

the Similarities subtest of the WISC Verbal Scale is generally well

known and accepted (Wechsler, 1949, 1974). In the present study, scaled

scores (Mean=10) were used throughout.

Verbal Fluency (VF). This is a modified form of the Verbal Fluency

test developed by Spreen and Benton (1965). A child was required to

name as many words as possible that begin with the letters F, A and S,

allowing one minute per letter. Scores were the total number of words

produced across all trials.

Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration (VMI). The VMI was

devised as a measure of the degree to which visual and motor behavior

are integrated in young children (Beery, 1967). It consists of a

series of 24 geometric forms to be copied with pencil and paper. The

forms are arranged in order of increasing difficulty. The copying of

geometric forms is stated to be well suited to the purpose of measuring

visual-motor integration because of the close correlation between visual

perception and the motoric expression that is required, and because,

unlike letter forms, geometric forms are equally familiar to children

of varying backgrounds. For purposes of this study an age-equivalent

score (in months) was used.









Recognition-Discrimination (RD). This visual-perceptual task

created by Small (1968) requires the child to match a geometric

stimulus design to one of four test figures. Three of the four were

rotated and/or similar in shape to the stimulus figure. The maximum

score for this task was 24.

Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT). Dunn (1965) designed this

test to provide an estimate of a child's verbal intelligence through

the measurement of "hearing vocabulary." He points to substantial corre-

lations of the PPVT with other measures of intelligence including WISC

Verbal, Performance and Full Scale IQ's, Stanford-Binet IQ's and Cali-

fornia Test of Mental Maturity IQ's. Accuracy for a wide range of

mental abilities and for normal clinical groups is claimed on the

basis of a large number of studies.

However, recent evidence suggests that IQ scores for certain

groups, including disadvantaged children and, perhaps, the learning

disabled, may show marked changes over time. A comprehensive longitu-

dinal study by Van De Riet and Resnick (1973) examined the effects of

an early childhood intervention program upon the development and

achievement of children from poverty backgrounds. On two measures of

IQ (WISC-R and Stanford-Binet) children who had participated in a

"learning to learn" program showed mean improvements of greater than

15 points over the course of 5 years. This finding seems to support,

in part, the widespread clinical practice of "adding points" to obtain

best estimates of the IQ's of culturally deprived, emotionally disturbed

and educationally handicapoed children. Dunn (1965) acknowledges that

while the PPVT has considerable face validity as a measure of "hearing

vocabulary" or even expressive language, its items do not provide a

comprehensive measure of intellectual functioning.










Therefore, when a test having the characteristics of the PPVT is

used to generate IQ scores, caution must be exercised in interpreting

the results. In the present study the IQ scores should be regarded

more as an estimate of current verbal-intellectual performance than

of the more abstract concept of an innate ability.

Socioeconomic Status (SES). The measure of socioeconomic status

was obtained from teacher ratings on a dichotomous scale and was scored

as either (1) low, or (2) average or above. This is admittedly a crude

measure, of questionable validity and reliability, but suitable for

discerning gross differences in cultural background.

Neurological Examinations (Neuro). These examinations were given

by pediatric residents under the direction of Dr. John Ross of the

University of Florida during year 4 of the longitudinal study of Satz

et al. (1977). It consisted of the following: (1) a general exam which

assessed cranial nerves, motor responses, sensation, reflexes and cere-

bellar functioning; (2) a special exam to evaluate fine and gross motor

functioning, right-left discrimination and eye tracking; and (3) an

examination of gross body anomalies or stigmata of the head, eyes, ears,

mouth and feet. Each examination was conducted without concurrent data

on the child and assignment was made to one of three categories (affected,

borderline-equivocal or normal) on the basis of overall clinical judge-

ment and component numerical scores.















CHAPTER IV
RESULTS

Phase I

The initial cluster solution was obtained by examining the com-

position of individual clusters at each stage of the clustering process.

As the analysis decreased the total number of clusters by fusing the

most similar entities two at a time, inspection of the mean reading,

spelling and arithmetic scores revealed the character of each cluster.

In this analysis it was found that a 12-cluster solution yielded the

most distinctive pattern of subgroups.

Tracing the clustering process exposed the trend of the data to

form clusters, which, by virtue of their mean reading and spelling

scores, could be arranged in scalar fashion. Mean arithmetic scores

for the clusters proved more variable. Solutions with more than 12

clusters served only to generate clusters with additional intermediate

mean values for reading, spelling and arithmetic scores. Solutions

consisting of less than 12 clusters resulted in the absorption of

clusters of potential interest because of their unique patterns of

scores. Additionally, a further reduction in the number of clusters

caused the range of achievement patterns to begin to regress towards

the overall sample mean.

Subjecting the initial cluster array to the relocation and fusion

procedure improved the clarity of individual clusters and confirmed that

the 12-cluster solution was near optimal for this study. Table 1 con-

tains the 12-cluster solution in the form of the mean WRAT reading, spell-

ing and arithmetic scores for each cluster and for the total sample.

35









The small number of individuals who made up clusters 10, 11 and

12 (N=6) led to considering these clusters as "outliers." The com-

ponent individuals all obtained extremely high (and deviant) reading

scores. Examination of the cluster fusion process revealed that no

other entities were clustered with any of these clusters. Similarly,

these "outliers" resisted incorporation into larger clusters until the

4-cluster solution. Following the recommendation of Everitt (1974),

these individuals (N=6) were dropped from further analysis.

Phase II

The remaining clusters (achievement subgroups) were then examined

for statistical differences on the clustering and dependent variables.

An analysis of variance revealed no significant age differences between

subgroups, F (8, 221) = .57, p>.81.

WRAT Scores

Although the total sample reading mean closely approximated the

WRAT standardization mean (Table 1), means for spelling and arithmetic

were 11 months below standardization norms (Jastak and Jastak, 1976).

As predicted, reading, spelling and arithmetic scores were highly corre-

lated for the total sample (Table 2).

A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) on WRAT reading,

spelling and arithmetic scores obtained an overall significant effect

for subgroup (Hotelling trace = 16.01, Fpproximation (24, 653) = 145.22,

2< .001). This finding justified the application of univariate analyses

for each of the variables. Individual analyses of variance yielded

significant effects for subgroup on WRAT reading, F (8, 221) = 199.76,

p< .0001, on WRAT spelling, F (8, 221) = 157.59, p< .0001, and on WRAT

arithmetic, F (8, 221) = 148.00, p< .0001.









Means for each WRAT variable are reported by subgroup in Table 1.

Tests of significance for differences between means revealed that in

only two instances for reading, and two for spelling, were pairs of

means not significantly different from each other (Duncan's pro-

cedure, p< .05). For arithmetic scores only one pair of means did not

significantly differ. These comparisons tend to confirm the appro-

priateness of the cluster solution and point to the unique character

of individual subgroups.

Subgroups 1 and 2 both obtained superior scores in reading, but

subgroup 2 exhibited only average performance in spelling and was

further distinguished by below-average scores in arithmetic. Subgroup 3

achieved high reading scores and average spelling and arithmetic scores.

Subgroup 4 emerged as a group with adequate reading and spelling scores,

but substandard performance in arithmetic. Subgroup 5 constituted a

unique group by virtue of its average reading, below average spelling

and severely depressed arithmetic scores. Subgroup 6's performance in

all areas was, perhaps, the most nearly average of all the subgroups.

At the lower end of the achievement spectrum, subgroups 7, 8 and

9 each contained a large number of children. Reading and spelling

scores for these subgroups could be arranged according to decreasing

levels of performance. Arithmetic scores were below average for all

three groups, although the mean scores of other subgroups (2, 4 and 5)

were in several instances as deficient. The overall achievement of

subgroups 8 and 9 was sufficiently depressed as to suggest that at

least these children suffer significant difficulties in learning.









Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test

An analysis of variance revealed a significant effect for subgroup

F (8, 217) = 10.46, p <.0001. Post hoc comparisons of IQ means (Dun-

can's procedure, e <.05) showed a strong ordering effect on the sub-

groups, in much the same manner as the WRAT reading scores (Table 3).

Subgroup PPVT means formed a chain from highest to lowest with adjacent

means not differing significantly from each other. Notably, no sub-

group obtained a mean IQ less than 90 and the total sample mean

(102.69) closely approximated the mean value reported for the PPVT

standardization sample.

Language and Perceptual-Motor Variables

Pearson product-moment correlations of all variables are presented

in Table 2 for the total sample. A multivariate analysis of variance

on WISC Similarities, Verbal Fluency, Test of Visual-Motor Integration

and Recognition-Discrimination revealed an overall significant effect

for subgroup (Hotelling trace = .76, Fon (4, 225) = 42.94,
-approximation
p <.0001). Individual analyses of variance yielded significant effects

for subgroup on all dependent variables: WISC Similarities, F (1, 228)

= 114.53, p <.0001, Verbal Fluency, F (1, 228) = 54.18, p< .0001, Test

of Visual-Motor Integration, F (1, 228) = 73.52, p <.0001 and Recogni-

tion-Discrimination, F (1, 228) = 29.96, p< .0001.

Similarities. Subgroup means for Similarities scaled scores along

with Duncan's Multi-Range Tests (p <.05) are reported in Table 3. The

relative performance of the subgroups is graphically represented in

Figure 1. While the highest and lowest reading groups tended also to

obtain the highest and lowest Similarities scores respectively, subgroup

6's performance was not statistically distinguishable from the two











TABLE 3


Mean PPVT, WISC Similarities, Verbal Fluency, VMI and Recognition-Discrimination
Performance by Achievement Subgroups


SUBGROUP N PPVT*


1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

Sample


116.77

117.93

110.27

110.12

99.08

102.64

103.15

96.66

90.52

102.69


SIM*


14.77

15.13

12.44

12.04

10.58

13.73

10.85

8.88

8.00

10.90


VMI*


35.23

30.81

32.84

30.08

28.42

30.64

30.05

23.79

19.79

27.65


135.08

121.56

125.20

117.24

87.25

119.00

102.10

95.27

87.18

105.74


19.61

18.88

19.84

18.68

16.42

18.64

18.69

16.80

16.30

17.96


* Means followed by the same letter are not significantly
(Duncan's Multi-Range Test, Alpha level = .05)


different within variables










SIM* VF*


1 2 3


4 6 7


589


Subgroups within the same box did not differ significantly for that variable.




Figure 1. Configuration of Achievement Suboroup Means for PPVT, WISC Similarities,
Verbal Fluency, VHI, and Recocnition-Discrimination


PPVT*


VMI* RD*









highest groups. Subgroups 8 and 9 produced the lowest levels of per-

formance on this variable.

Verbal Fluency. Subgroup means for the Verbal Fluency variable

with post hoc tests are presented in Table 3. Inspection of Figure 1

for this variable reveals a large number of subgroups with statistically

similar means (subgroups 1, 2, 3, 4, 6 and 7). Subgroup 5 was charac-

terized by performance which fell between that of this large group and

that of subgroups 8 and 9. The uniquely poor performance of subgroup

9 was particularly striking.

Test of Visual-Motor Integration. The VMI mean developmental age-

equivalents for subgroups, with post hoc comparisons, are reported in

Table 3. The relationship of subgroup means is depicted in Figure 1.

While the association of subgroups 1, 2 and 3 with higher scores and

subgroups 8 and 9 with the lowest scores persisted, subgroup 6 again

performed in a manner similar to the highest subgroups. In contrast,

subgroup 5 obtained scores among the lowest for the sample. The total

sample mean of 105.74 months represents a value substantially below

that expected for the sample whose mean age was 130.01.

Recognition-Discrimination. Inspection of Table 3 and Figure 1

reveals a significant difference between the performance of subgroups

1, 2, 3, 4, 6 and 7 and that of subgroups 5, 8 and 9. This dichotomous

division suggested the similarity of,subgroup 5 to that of the lowest

achievement groups.

Socioeconomic Status

The chi-square test applied to the frequency distribution of this

variable reflected a significant relationship between socioeconomic

status and subgroup, X = 28.14, p<.0004. Only 18 percent of the









children of the total sample were rated as having low socioeconomic

status. Subgroups 8 and 9 showed greater representation of the low

ratings (34 percent each). The distribution of "low" versus "average

or above" ratings is presented in Table 4.

Neurological Status

The chi-square test for the independence of the distribution of

neurological status by subgroup confirmed the significant relationship
2
between these variables, X = 74.92, p< .0001. Inspection of Table 5

reveals that the academic achievement of subgroups was generally asso-

ciated with frequency of positive neurological findings. The two

subgroups with the lowest achievement scores (subgroups 8 and 9) also

had the highest percentage of "affected" neurological ratings. Subgroup

9, in fact, included no children who were judged "normal." The percentage

of "affected" and "equivocal" ratings for subgroup 5 suggested the simi-

larity of this group to the two lowest achievement subgroups with

respect to frequency of neurological symptoms.

The total sample showed an overall high proportion of neurological

examinations rated "affected" (48 percent). However, it should be noted

that there were 68 missing values from the distribution and that some

cells were only sparsely represented. Therefore, all results pertain-

ing to this variable must be interpreted with caution.

Phase III

Cluster Analysis

The 89 children of subgroups 8 and 9 were selected for further

examination by virtue of their overall low achievement scores. The mean

WRAT reading, spelling and arithmetic discrepancy scores for the com-

bined subgroups were sufficiently depressed that they could legitimately

be regarded as learning disabled (Table 6).











TABLE 4


Socioeconomic Status by Achievement Subgroups


N MISSING
VALUE


L3 0

L6 0

?5 0

25 0

12 1

11 0

39 0

56 0

33 1

30 2


LOW AVERAGE OR ABOVE
% of % of
Frequency %of Frequency of
Subgroup frequency Subgroup


0 0 13 100

2 12 14 88

0 0 25 100

3 12 22 88

2 18 9 82

1 9 10 91

3 8 36 92

19 34 37 66

11 34 21 66

41 18 187 82


SUBGROUP



1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

Sample











TABLE 5


Neurological Ratings by Achievement Subgroups


NORMAL
SUBGROUP N MISSING % of
VALUE Frequency Subgroup


EQUIVOCAL
% of
Frequency Subgroup
Subgroup


AFFECTED
% of
Frequency Subgroup
Subgroup


1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

Sample











TABLE 6


Mean PPVT, WISC Similarities, Verbal Fluency, VMI and Recognition-Discrimination
Performance by Learning Disabled Subtypes


SUBTYPE N PPVT* SIM*


1 15 105.73 A 10.93 A


2 14 101.79 A 10.14 A


3 23 97.35 A 9.78 A


4 32 86.84 B 6.31 B


Outliers 5 74.20 5.60


Total 230 102.69 10.90
Sample

Means followed by the same letter are not
(Duncan's Multi-Range Test, Alpha level =


VF*


35.87


18.50


24.21


18.06


10.60


VMI*


108.20


110.43


83.52


86.47


71.00


27.65 105.74


significantly different within
.05)


RD*


19.53


18.71


16.88


14.96


8.00


17.96


variables









Clustering these 89 children on the basis of their WISC Simi-

larities, Verbal Fluency, Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration

and Recognition-Discrimination scores resulted in a 5-cluster optimum

solution. The solution was identified by tracing the value of the

total similarity coefficient through each stage of the clustering pro-

cess and by examining the composition and "N" of each cluster at each

stage. A dramatic shift in the rate of change of the total similarity

coefficient occurred during the transition from 5 to 4 clusters. This

shift suggested that two relatively heterogeneous clusters, while the

most similar of those clusters remaining, had been forced together.

Inspection of the 5 and 4-cluster solutions confirmed that two relatively

large clusters (N's of 15 and 19) had been fused to generate the 4-

cluster solution. In contrast a 6-cluster solution contained an

additional cluster with only 3 members.

Subjecting the 5-cluster solution to the relocation procedure re-

sulted in the formation of 4 clusters with a significant number of

members (15, 14, 32 and 23). The fifth cluster of only 5 individuals

was not altered by any of the relocation passes, nor was it able to

be combined with any other cluster. These individuals, because of their

small number and because of their extremely deviant low scores on the

clustering variables (Table 6), were designated as "outliers" for pur-

poses of further analysis. The 4 remaining clusters were tentatively

identified as subtypes of the learning disabled population.

WRAT Scores

The reduced sample of learning disabled children (N=84) obtained

a mean reading score of -24.06 months (SD=9.07), a mean spelling score

of -29.56 months (SD=6.49) and a mean arithmetic score of -20.55









(SD=6.29). A multivariate analysis of variance did not yield a signi-

ficant effect for subtype (Hotelling trace = .20, Fapprox (9, 230)
-approximation
= 1.72, p >.08). Thus, subtypes could not be differentiated on the

basis of academic performance.

Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test

An analysis of variance yielded a significant effect for subtype,

F (3, 80) = 9.87, p <.0001. Post hoc tests (Duncan's procedure, p <.05)

revealed that only subtype 4 produced a significantly deviant mean

(Table 6). The other three subtypes obtained means which approximated

both the total sample mean as well as the national norm.

Language and Perceptual-Motor Variables

A multivariate analysis of variance showed a significant effect for

subtype on WISC Similarities, Verbal Fluency, Developmental Test of

Visual-Motor Integration and Recognition-Discrimination (Hotelling trace

= 4.77, F approximation (12, 227) = 30.09, e <.0001). Individual analyses

of variance confirmed the significant effect of subtype on each of these

measures: WISC Similarities, F (3, 80) = 31.98, p< .0001, Verbal

Fluency, F (3, 80) = 48.65, p <.0001, Test of Visual-Motor Integration,

F (3, 80) = 22.15, p<.0001, and Recognition-Discrimination, F (3, 80)

= 25.51, P <.0001.

WISC Similarities. Means and Duncan's procedures, < .05, for

subtypes may be examined in Table 6. Subtypes 1, 2 and 3 obtained

statistically indistinguishable mean scaled scores, which were con-

sistent with the overall sample mean of 10.9, as well as the WISC

standardization value of 10. In contrast, subtype 4 showed a substan-

tially deficient performance (Mean=6.31).









Verbal Fluency. The results of post hoc comparisons between sub-

type means (Duncan's procedure, p <.05) revealed a diversity of per-

formances (Table 6). Surprisingly, subtype 2 produced low scores not

significantly different from those of subtype 4. Equally surprising

was the superior mean score of subtype 1, which was comparable to the

means of the highest achievement groups of the total sample of 230.

Subtype 3 obtained scores which were nearly average.

Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration. Subtype means

and tests of significant differences between these means (Duncan's pro-

cedure, p <.05) are reported in Table 6. On this variable subtypes 1

and 2 obtained means approximating the total sample mean and not sig-

nificantly different from each other. The means of subtypes 3 and 4

were likewise statistically equivalent. However, the performance of

these two latter subtypes suggested pronounced deficits in visual-motor

skill development.

Recognition-Discrimination. The means for subtypes 1 and 2 were

not significantly different as determined by the post hoc tests (Table

6). Their scores were substantially higher than the total sample mean

suggesting that these subtypes were unimpaired on this task. Subtypes

3 and 4 obtained mean scores substantially below that of subtypes 1 and

2, as well as below that of the total sample mean. On this variable

subtype 4 scored significantly below even the mean of subtype 3.

Socioeconomic Status

A chi-square test for independence of the subtype and socioeconomic
2
status variable distributions was non-significant, X3 = 4.42, > .21.

Thus, subtypes could not be differentiated from each other on the basis

of SES, although, as noted earlier, the learning disabled sample as a






49

whole showed a significantly greater number of low status children than

the higher achievement sample.

Neurological Status

The relationship of subtypes to neurological status was also found
non-significant, X = 8.55, >.20. While there was a high inci-
to be non-significant, X3 = 8.55, p >.20. While there was a high inci-

dence of "affected" ratings among all subtypes, Table 7 raises the

possibility that subtype 1 tends to be less neurologically impaired.

This hypothesis must remain speculative because of the large proportion

of missing values.











TABLE 7


Neurological Ratings by Learning Disabled Subtypes


NORMAL EQUIVOCAL AFFECTED
SUBTYPE N MISSING % of of % of
VALUE Subtype y Subtype ubtype


1 15 6 1 11 3 38 5 56


Subtype
Sample


14 2


23 6


32 11


84 25















CHAPTER V
DISCUSSION

A critical appraisal of research on learning disabilities must

conclude that there is a lack of consensus concerning almost every

aspect of this problem except its importance. Some reviewers (e.g.,

Benton, 1975; Applebee, 1971) have suggested that one major source of

the present state of theoretical disarray has been the unwarranted

assumption that learning disabled children constitute a homogeneous

population. Their position is supported by the diversity of reported

correlates of learning disability found in the current literature.

Similarly, no one of the numerous competing theories has been able to

account adequately for the myriad of defects present in these children.

Most researchers acknowledge that the end result of substandard

school achievement may be produced by such conditions as gross neuro-

logical defect, severe cultural or educational deprivation, impaired

intellect, or debilitating psychopathology. However, those learning

disabled children who suffer from none of these handicaps have defied

accurate classification, other than to consider them as cases of learn-

ing difficulties of unknown origin. One approach toward reducing the

ambiguity presented by these children has been to subsume them all

under a single label as, for example, "specific developmental dyslexia."

An alternative solution has been to group children according to a set

of predetermined, sometimes arbitrary, criteria. Both approaches appear

to force a premature structure upon the often complex patterns of per-

formance produced by these individuals. The present study represents a









preliminary effort to return to a basic stance of exploration in the

search for naturally-occurring subtypes among the learning disabled.

An endeavor of discovery should be as free from a prior restric-

tions as possible, while still proceeding in systematic fashion. This

project had as its primary goal the identification of subtypes through

empirical means. As such, no attempt was made to employ a single set of

theoretical constructs or constraints, nor to examine a select group

of children. The 236 children available for the study represented the

wide range of achievement occurring in the general school population.

While the ultimate focus was to be on learning disabled children, the

initial phases of the study sought to examine the performance character-

istics of children of all levels of academic ability. Learning disability

was here defined operationally by those children who clustered together

on the basis of similarly low achievement scores. The sample size, one

of the largest reported in the current literature, was felt to provide

ample opportunity for a variety of distinctive groupings to emerge.

Similarly, the measurement of academic performance was not confined

solely to reading competency. Although the vast majority of studies

have singled out reading disability as representative of problems in

learning, there is no firm evidence to suggest that failure in other

academic areas is not equally as significant. For example, the findings

of Rourke and Finlayson (1978) suggest that relatively deficient per-

formance in arithmetic is associated with a unique pattern of neuro-

psychological deficits. These deficits contrast with those displayed

by children whose reading and spelling scores are relatively inferior.

The authors speculate that differential patterns of academic achievement

may reflect differences in the integrity of the cortical mechanisms









which ultimately subserve them. At the same time, there is not suffi-

cient evidence to conclude that reading disability consistently occurs

as a singular deficit. Several studies (e.g., Cole and Kraft, 1964;

Ingram et al., 1970) have demonstrated that reading disability is most

frequently accompanied by other educational handicaps.

One of the most powerful features of the present study was the

manner in which subgroups were identified in the initial phase. With

the exception of Doehring and Hoshko (1977) most investigators have

relied upon the process of inspection alone or the use of "cut-off"

scores to select their disabled samples. When more than one criterion

measure is used, as was the case with the reading, spelling and arith-

metic scores employed here, the resulting complexity of the data set

effectively rules out the use of inspection techniques. In contrast,

the cluster analytic procedures employed in this study permitted the

data themselves to dictate the nature of the grouoings that emerged.

Cluster analysis represents a method tailor-made to discriminate simi-

larities of individuals along a variety of dimensions. While an

investigator must ultimately exercise judgement in determining which

cluster solution is empirically useful, he can be assured that a par-

ticular cluster array has been systematically and impartially derived.

Examination of the statistically generated clusters revealed a

configuration which could be regarded heuristically as a preliminary

achievement-based classification system. Since the final value of a

classification system rests upon its utility, it was necessary to

establish both the statistical and empirical validity of the subgroups.

Thus, analyses were conducted to determine if individual clusters were

associated with particular patterns of scores on the developmental,









intellectual, cultural or neurological variables. Through these

analyses the search for meaningful achievement-subgroups was conducted

in a particularly rigorous manner.

While the total sample size was certainly adequate (N=236), this

number represents only slightly more than a third of the original

population of 678 subjects studied in the longitudinal project of Satz

et al. (1977). Although no systematic selection process was applied

beforehand in choosing the subjects for this study, there is evidence

to suggest that they constitute a somewhat unusual sample. One indi-

cation of the deviancy of this group is the relatively substandard

spelling and arithmetic mean scores for the sample. Reading scores,

on the other hand, closely approximated the national norms reported by

Jastak and Jastak (1976). The mean discrepancy scores of -11.2 months

and 11 months in spelling and arithmetic respectively clearly represent

deficient performance, although not necessarily of pathological signi-

ficance.

One likely explanation for this finding is that the inferior scores

reflect the effects of differences between local educational standards

and those of the national standardization sample. However, if this

explanation is accepted, the question arises why the mean reading score

for the local sample was not also lower. The significant correlational

values between reading, spelling and arithmetic scores reported in

Table 2 for the total sample compare favorably with those found for

this age by Jastak and Jastak (1976). The sum of this evidence suggests

that the relationships among scores were stable across the range of

achievement and that a particular child's spelling and arithmetic per-

formance was likely to be inferior to his reading performance.









An immediate consequence of the sample's depressed mean spelling

and arithmetic scores is the dilemma of assigning descriptive labels

to individual achievement subgroups. Assignment of a label is contin-

gent upon whether local or national norms are used. For example, the

children of subgroup 5 could be classified according to local sample

means as roughly average in reading and spelling with an arithmetic

deficiency. According to national norms these same children would show

average reading, depressed spelling and severely impaired arithmetic

performance. There are valid arguments for each position. On the one

hand it could be claimed that these children certainly have not

achieved on a par with their national peers in spelling or arithmetic;

therefore, they represent a deficient group. On the other hand it could

be speculated that these children performed at an average level (in

spelling) given their educational and cultural opportunity. It is,

perhaps, more judicious to avoid the use of labels which imply absolute

levels of performance, particularly in the middle ranges of achievement.

Another disturbing finding of the present study is the large number

of children who showed significantly depressed scores in one or more

areas of achievement. Regardless of which interpretation is made con-

cerning the sample mean scores in spelling and arithmetic, the perform-

ance of subgroups 8 and 9, and probably subgroup 5, can clearly be

classed as deficient. The 89 children of subgroups 8 and 9 alone rep-

resent almost 38 percent of the total sample. This figure testifies

to the significance of learning disabilities to public education. Un-

doubtedly, this figure is increased by the inclusion of the additional

areas-of spelling and arithmetic. This fact may, in part, account for

the discrepancy between the 38 percent found here and more conservative









estimates of approximately 15 percent (e.g., Kline, 1972) in studies in

which only severe reading disability was considered.

Accompanying the depressed scores in two of the three academic

areas there are two other significant characteristics of this sample in

need of discussion. First, the mean developmental-age scores of the

total sample obtained by the Test of Visual-Motor Integration (105.70

months) represented a deficit in expected performance of greater than

2 years relative to the mean chronological age of the sample (130.01

months). No satisfactory explanation is suggested by these data and

any interpretations must remain highly speculative. However, the possible

association of depressed arithmetic and spelling scores with inferior

visual-motor performance is certainly worthy of further study in light

of the findings of Rourke and Finlayson (1978).

A final major feature of the sample was the high incidence of

"affected" neurological ratings (approximately 48 percent). Taking into

account that the bulk of these ratings were obtained by children from the

large low-achievement subgroups, this percentage is, nonetheless, con-

siderably higher than estimates reported in most other current research.

Cole and Kraft (1964), for example, found that only 50 percent of their

learning disabled subjects showed evidence of neurological abnormalities.

It is noteworthy that nearly a third of the subjects in the present

study did not receive neurological examinations. Furthermore, the type

of examination conducted included extensive procedures designed to detect

even subtle impairments in neurological performance, thereby enhancing

the chances for the assignment of an abnormal rating. Thus, while there

is not reason to conclude that the ratings were inaccurate, it is probable

that their significance is not equivalent to that of other studies.









Within the total sample, the 9 subgroups generated by the cluster

analysis each exhibited a unique configuration of WRAT reading, spelling

and arithmetic scores. It is unlikely that any two subgroups could be

merged without losing potentially valuable information concerning the

structure of the achievement data. The children of subgroup 1 were

clearly superior, not only in their reading, spelling and arithmetic

achievement, but also in their performance on each of the language and

perceptual-motor tasks. None of these children were judged to have

low socioeconomic background and only one child was given an "affected"

neurological rating.

Subgroup 2 was characterized by a dramatic discrepancy between their

excellent reading scores and their undistinguished spelling and arithmetic

scores. However, this group's performance on the dependent measures

was not statistically different from that of the most superior group.

There was some suggestion of a trend toward relatively lower per-

ceptual-motor scores (e.g., on the VMI), although the meaning of this

trend is difficult to ascertain.

The achievement scores of subgroup 3 identified them as good readers

with no other demonstrable academic deficits. Similar to subgroups 1

and 2 on most tasks (Verbal Fluency, VMI and Recognition-Discrimination),

their lower Similarities scores implied a slight decrement in verbal-

abstractive abilities relative to the two higher groups. The lower

arithmetic scores (compared to spelling and reading) of subgroup 4 re-

sulted in a pattern similar to subgroup 2, but at a lower achievement

level. Again there was the hint of a relationship between depressed

arithmetic scores and perceotual-motor performance. At the same time

their above-average reading scores were accompanied by performance on the









PPVT and language measures which was not significantly different from

subgroup 3.

The performance of subgroup 5 is worthy of careful scrutiny. These

children obtained average reading scores, but showed depressed spelling

and severely retarded arithmetic scores. It is the performance of

this group of 12 children which cautions against limiting the study of

learning disabilities to children with reading problems. Obviously,

this group would not be singled out for special attention on the basis

of their reading scores and yet in several respects their performance

is indistinguishable from the two lowest groups of achievers. While

their PPVT, Similarities and Verbal Fluency scores were nearly average,

this subgroup obtained extremely low scores on both VMI and Recognition-

Discrimination. Although there were statistical similarities of this

subgroup to subgroups 8 and 9 in all areas, the pronounced deficits on

these perceptual-motor tasks clearly identifies these children as suffer-

ing from significant developmental problems. By way of confirmation

55 percent of these children were assigned ratings of "affected" upon

neurological examination. The combination of depressed spelling and

arithmetic scores exemplifies the interpretive dilemma referred to

earlier. While the mean arithmetic score certainly represents a deficit

of some magnitude, the spelling scores must be evaluated in light of the

overall sample spelling mean. Speculation regarding the underlying

mechanisms involved would be hazardous at this point. However, future

studies would do well to consider such children in addition to the poor

readers.

The reading, spelling and arithmetic scores of subgroup 6 were the

most nearly average and unremarkable of all the subgroups. However, on









4 of the 5 dependent measures their performance closely resembled that

of the two superior subgroups. Subgroup 6 differed significantly from

subgroups 1 and 2 only in their mean PPVT IQ's. The scores of subgroup

7 on all dependent test measures most closely approximated the means for

the total sample. It is likely that both their reading, spelling and

arithmetic achievement, and their developmental performance represents

an intermediate value between those of the average and deficient achieve-

ment subgroups.

The differences in achievement between subgroups 8 and 9 were

largely ones of degree. Both subgroups are readily recognizable as

experiencing serious difficulties in all three academic areas. Their

performance on the language and perceptual-motor tasks confirmed that

these subjects suffered serious deficiencies across a range of develop-

mental skills. These findings coupled with the unusually high propor-

tion of positive neurological examinations and greater number of "low"

socioeconomic ratings underscore the pronounced vulnerability of these

children to difficulties in all facets of their educational lives. It

is significant that these problems emerged in the absence of grossly

substandard IQ scores. Both subgroups 8 and 9 obtained PPVT IQ's

which fell within the "average learners" category relative to the

national norms.

Data provided by the Phase I and II analyses raise serious ques-

tions regarding current methods in research involving disabled children

and, inferentially, the assumptions which underlie these methods. The

failure of the analyses to identify a unique reading-spelling group

(or even a group deficient in both reading and spelling) suggests that

"dyslexia" as an isolated syndrome, may be a rarer occurrence among

children than previously supposed.









The failure to find an isolated reading-disabled group among the

achievement subgroups is not surprising upon close inspection of the

Rourke and Finlayson (1978) sample. These investigators were readily

able to identify their Group 3 children on the basis of "average or

above" reading scores and relatively deficient arithmetic performance.

In contrast, their Group 2 children were only relatively adept in

arithmetic compared to their performance in reading and spelling.

Group 2's level of arithmetic achievement was substantially depressed.

Furthermore, Group 2 children did not differ from Group 1 children (who

showed deficiencies in all 3 areas) on any of the 16 dependent measures.

Thus, it is doubtful that Rourke and Finlayson were able to isolate

a "pure" verbally-deficient group in the manner of Groups 1 and 3.

Both the subgroup 5 children of this study and the Group 3 children

of Rourke and Finlayson (1978) displayed impaired development of im-

portant non-verbal skills, though they had acquired adequate language

skills. While deficient perceptual-motor performance effectively dis-

tinguished subgroup 5, no such differential pattern was found for the

large numbers of children in subgroups 8 and 9. Rather, these children

tended to perform poorly on all measures of development. It should be

emphasized that these results were obtained through sampling at a single

age and, therefore, cannot be comprehensively applied to a number of

developmental issues. However, these results tend to challenge the

"lag" hypothesis encountered in some current theories (e.g., Satz et al.,

1977) which predicts that learning disabled children eventually "catch

up" on certain earlier developing skills, (e.g., visual-perceptual and

cross-modal sensory integration). The magnitude of the deficits on the

VMI and Recognition-Discrimination variables for subgroups 8 and 9









suggest that at an age when language-conceptual difficulties should

begin to predominate, significant perceptual-motor problems tend to

persist.

However, it should be recalled that most developmental theories

of learning disability have been based on and applied to what is po-

tentially a quite heterogeneous population. In the example cited above,

it is conceivable that within subgroups 8 and 9 certain children could

show deficits exclusively in language, having made up deficits in other

areas. Other children may have demonstrated deficient language and

perceptual-motor skills throughout their development. The final phase

of this study was designed to provide a preliminary basis for further

investigation into these possibilities. If it could be demonstrated

that certain distinctive patterns of deficits exist at the age addressed

in this study, efforts could then be directed toward examining the

developmental processes which produced these patterns.

The simultaneous emergence of the four cluster-subtypes in the

final cluster analysis constituted one of the unique findings of this

study. It appears that no previous classification systems have ade-

quately encompassed the range of logically possible combinations of

deficits which may be represented in the learning disabled population.

The present data suggest that not only are such combinations possible,

but that they actually occur. Thus, the final cluster configuration

included: (1) children who showed deficits in neither language nor

perceptual-motor development; (2) children with impairment in both areas;

(3) children with language-related deficiencies; and (4) children with

deficits in perception and perceptual-motor integration.









The differences between these subtypes were not found to be asso-

ciated with the severity of their academic failure. For example, the

relatively superior IQ and Verbal Fluency scores of subtype 1 did not

save these children from the same dismal achievement as that of subtype

4, who demonstrated considerably more limited resources in these areas.

Neither could individual subtypes be discriminated on the basis of their

impoverished socioeconomic background, although the measure employed

in this study was admittedly crude. Similarly, the high rate of ab-

normal neurological findings for the total group precluded indentifi-

cation of any single "neurological subtype" (c.f. Owen et al., 1971).

This finding is in accordance with Mattis et al. (1975) who found that

their dyslexic subjects could not be differentiated on the basis of

presence or absence of brain damage. Nonetheless, the subtypes deline-

ated in this analysis exhibited statistically significant differences

along dimensions which have considerable relevance to current research

and theory.

Little controversy surrounds the existence of subtype 4. This

group of children typically obtains the lowest scores on almost all pro-

ficiency measures employed in the classroom. Denckla (1972) has esti-

mated that fully 70 percent of the learning disabled population should

be classified in the "mixed or unclear" category. In the present study

the 32 children of this subtype represented within the reduced sample

the poorest performers on all variables. Their performance produced the

only IQ's to fall outside the average range. On no language or perceptual-

motor variable did their performance approach the mean of the total

sample (N=230).









In many respects subtype 4 resembled the children categorized by

Yule and Rutter (1976) as suffering from "general reading backwardness."

Both these groups showed depressed IQ scores, demonstrated inferior

perceptual-constructional abilities and exhibited significant speech

and language impairment. The children in the "general reading backward-

ness" group tended to have a higher incidence of neurological problems

and social disadvantage than the group identified as "specific reading

retarded." However, they also showed a better prognosis for future

academic achievement in all areas except arithmetic. This latter find-

ing.by Yule and Rutter cautions against prematurely discriminating against

these children in terms of the focus of future research, as well as

intervention.

Subtype 2 emerged as a distinctive group by virtue of significantly

depressed Verbal Fluency scores. This deficient performance was highly

circumscribed and did not extend into other language measures, including

WISC Similarities or the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Neither was

there evidence that these children experienced lags in perceptual-motor

skill acquisition. Only in verbal fluency did their scores fall sub-

stantially below the means of the total sample.

Benton (1975) has identified generalized language disability as

one of the persistent defects reported in reading disabled children.

He points to the large number of studies which have noted difficulties

in verbal production as a crucial obstacle to the development of ade-

quate reading skills. In the classification system of Cole and Kraft

(1964) a group described as "dyslexia with general language defect"

contained the largest number of individuals. The criteria which de-

fined their group were (1) a history of retarded speech development and









(2) abnormalities of expressive or receptive speech. Interestingly,

the group was composed entirely of boys and fully 84 percent had a

positive family history of learning difficulties.

Mattis et al. (1975) similarly stressed the importance of verbal

retrieval problems in both the brain-damaged and nonbrain-damaged

dyslexics who composed their "language disorder" group. Like the

children of subtype 2, the "language disorder" children showed im-

peded verbal production, but intact visuo-constructional skills. In

contrast to the Mattis results,subtype 2, characterized by deficiencies

in verbal fluency in the present study, was the least common of the

four subtypes. Subtype 2 also resembled the category of "specific

reading retardation" identified by Yule and Rutter (1975) as exhibiting

deficits only in speech and language. Both groups obtained remarkably

similar mean IQ estimates falling solidly in the average range.

There appears to be a strong consensus regarding the existence of

a group of children whose deficiencies lie almost exclusively in the

area of language development. However, it is presently unclear whether

the disorder is manifested from earliest childhood or is developmentally

preceded by deficiencies in sensory, perceptual or motor skills. Like-

wise, the evidence is inconclusive as to which specific language-speech

functions are compromised. For instance, some studies (e.g., Cole and

Kraft, 1964) suggest that the disorder encompasses both receptive and

expressive language dimensions.

Benton (1975) in his review of the defects reported to be associated

with learning disability has commented that the role of visuo-perceptive

factors has been overrated. Part of his rationale rests on the observa-

tion that these defects do not generally persist into adulthood.









Nonetheless, numerous studies (e.g., Cole and Kraft, 1964; Ingram,

1966; Denckla, 1972; and Mattis et al., 1975) have concluded that a small

but persistent group of children suffer relatively circumscribed defi-

cits in perceptual-motor development including the specific problems of

visuo-spatial agnosia, constructional apraxia, graphomotor dyscoordina-

tion and poor visual-motor integration. Such a population was represented

in the present study by subtype 3.

In contrast to the majority of other studies, subtype 3 accounted

for a relatively large proportion of the learning disabled subjects

(approximately 27 percent). These children exhibited strikingly inferior

performance on both the VMI and the Recognition-Discrimination task.

Their PPVT and Similarities mean scores were not significantly different

from those of subtypes 1 and 2, nor from those of the original achieve-

ment sample.

The emergence of subtype 3 as a distinctive and well-established

group again presents a serious challenge for explanation to "develop-

mental lag" theories which predict that perceptual-motor deficits are

characteristic of much younger disabled learners. These youngsters pur-

portedly should make up these deficits only to lag behind in later

years in language skill development. Although developmental trends are

more appropriately addressed through longitudinal designs, the present

results suggest that language and perceptual-motor decrements in

performance may be present separately, or together (as in subtypes 2,

3 and 4) at the same chronological age.

Perhaps the most surprising group to emerge in the analysis was

subtype 1. While their poor achievement was not distinguishable from

the other subtypes, its source is difficult to infer because there









appeared to be no functional correlates to their disabilities. With

the possible exception of the VMI their scores on each variable were

at least average. Although the proportion of "affected" neurological

ratings was not statistically different from that of other subtypes,

the 56 percent figure for subtype 1 is comparable to the total sample

mean of 48 percent and to that of achievement subgroup 5. On the basis

of verbal-conceptual development the children of subtype 1 would be

difficult to suspect of having learning problems. In fact because of

their superior verbal fluency, they might create an initial impression

in educational situations of having far greater general abilities.

The significance of the mean VMI score for subtype 1 (108.20

months) is difficult to assess. On the one hand this value reflects

nearly a 2-year lag behind chronological age. On the other the subtype

1 mean slightly exceeded the mean for the total sample. In the context

of the present design it could be argued that relative to other disabled

subtypes (3 and 4) subtype 1 performed demonstrably better.

Since none of the developmental, intellectual or neurological mea-

sures clearly discriminated subtype 1, it was hypothesized that perhaps

this subtype was composed of individuals who suffered from significant

psychopathology or motivational problems. Numerous investigators have

postulated the close association of reading disability with emotional

disturbance and personality dysfunction (e.g., Owen et al., 1971; Rourke,

1975). The clinical impressions of Rabinovitch et al. (1954) suggest

that the nature of these associations may vary with the type of reading

difficulty. The administration of the Children's Personality Question-

naire (Porter and Cattell, 1972) permitted the measurement of 14 inde-

pendent factors, or traits, presumed to underlie the normal personality.









These 14 raw factor scores, employed as dependent measures in a multi-

variate analysis of variance, provided a basis for comparing the sub-

types. However, the analysis failed to reveal differences between sub-

types along any of the 14 dimensions (Hotelling trace = .59, F approxi-

mation (42, 167) = .78, p> .82).

Confronted with the sum of this puzzling evidence, subtype 1 must

be considered as composed of children with truly "unexpected learning

difficulties." The current research literature has not addressed this

subtype and contains few clues to the deficiencies which account for

the failure of this substantial number of children. It remains for

future studies to explore additional areas of childhood development

in the search to validate and, ultimately, to explain their retarded

achievement.

Overall, the results of the present study demonstrate the need for

investigators studying the learning problems of children to specify

carefully the populations selected for scrutiny. Ultimately, the par-

ticular achievement subgroups identified here may not prove to be

definitive. However, there can be little doubt that a considerable

variety of achievement-score relationships are represented by signifi-

cant numbers of children. Such patterns are to be found at all levels

of achievement from superior to disabled. Further study of all achieve-

ment levels may provide insight into the general principles and compo-

nent skills involved in the mastery of material from different academic

areas. The conclusion of Rourke and Finlayson (1978), that patterns,

rather than levels of achievement, are the salient feature in the pro-

duction of learning disability, may well be validated through such

studies. It should then be possible to generate hypotheses regarding

possible underlying mechanisms of learning disability with far greater

precision.









The emergence of four unique and meaningful subtypes supports the

position advanced by some investigators that the population of disabled

learners is quite heterogeneous. In accordance with Applebee's (1971)

model it appears that poor achievement may be simultaneously associated

with each of several different patterns of deficits. The data from

this study admit the possibility that substandard learning achievement

may occur in the presence of (1) verbal fluency difficulties alone,

(2) deficient perceptual-motor development alone, (3) deficits in neither

skill or (4) defects in both skills. Hypotheses which address only one

of these dimensions are likely to yield inconclusive results when

applied indiscriminately across all groups. For instance, a theory

which stresses exclusively the role of language defects in learning dis-

abilities will be hard-pressed to account for the performance of chil-

dren of subtypes 1 and 3.

Building upon the foundation of the present project, future research

can next address the developmental parameters which may characterize

each of the subtypes. In the manner of Zigler (1969) it might be found

that some subtypes conform most nearly to a lag model, others to a

defect model and still others to a difference model. One approach to

this issue might be to trace the performance of the present individual

subtypes across age ranges and to compare their developmental character-

istics with those of a "normal" control group.

Another avenue of exploration would be to utilize cluster analytic

procedures to identify learning disabled children who showed similar

patterns of development, as reflected by their scores on selected per-

formance variables at different chronological ages. The composition of

the resulting clusters could then be examined to determine which






69


characteristics distinguished each group. These subgroups could then

be compared with normal or superior achievement groups using multi-

variate procedures.

It is apparent that the use of modern clustering techniques greatly

facilitates the examination of the hidden structure of complex data sets.

This capability, applied to the search for the multitude of dimensions

which underlie children's learning problems, offers considerable promise

for solid advances in the understanding of this crucial social and

educational problem.














APPENDIX
CLUSTER ANALYSIS

Cluster analysis is a statistical procedure which facilitates the

creation of a classification scheme. Succinctly stated, the task is to

group N objects or individuals into g classes, given that each object

is measured on each of p variables. Everitt (1974) has identified at

least seven possibilities for the use of clustering techniques in-

cluding: (1) finding a true typology, (2) model fitting, (3) prediction

based on groups, (4) hypothesis testing, (5) data exploration, (6)

hypothesis generating and (7) data reduction. However, the flexibility

and scope of cluster analytic methods have not been matched as yet by

their precision or power. Blashfield (1976) has examined the accuracy

of four agglomerative heirarchical methods and found that there was

considerable variance in the adequacy of cluster solutions generated

by these methods. He acknowledges that the literature on cluster

analysis is still in its infancy and suggests caution in the adoption

of a particular classification result.

Although cluster analysis was originally proposed some years

earlier the procedure did not gain widespread notice until the advent

of high speed computers. One of the earliest works was a book on

numerical taxonomy in biology by Sokol and Sneath (1963). Blash-

field's (1976) review of the relevant literature reveals a veritable

explosion of research addressing the theory and application of cluster

analysis since the early sixties. He concludes that no single theory

or method has gained ascendancy.









Perhaps the most popular methods in current use are the hier-

archical procedures. Hierarchical methods attempt to form homogeneous

groups by systematically analyzing a similarity/dissimilarity matrix.

The matrix is constructed by computing the similarity of every entity

to every other entity. Various measures are available to indicate

similarity including the squared euclidean distance measure used in

both clustering phases of this study. Examples of other measures

frequently used are product-moment correlations and error sum of squares.

Following the formation of a similarity matrix, the individuals

are then assigned to clusters using one of several linkage methods.

Linkage methods refer to the mechanism used to join entities to form

clusters. For Phase I of this study an average linkage method (some-

times called the unweightedd pair-wise group mean average linkage

method") was selected for its property of allowing "nonconformist"

clusters to form (Blashfield, 1976). This method requires that before

an entity can join a cluster it must achieve a given level of simi-

larity with the average of the members already belonging to the cluster.

In Phase III a minimum variance method (Ward's method; Wishart,

1975) was employed. Although not suitable for all applications (as

in Phase I) this method has the advantage of optimizing an objective

statistic (the error sum of squares among the members of each cluster)

and is, perhaps, the most generally reliable and accurate of available

methods.

Two problems present themselves in connection with the hierar-

chical clustering methods. The first results from the process by which

clusters are formed in hierarchical fashion. An individual, once

placed in a given cluster, is not able to be reassigned to a








later-forming cluster, even if its similarity to the latter cluster

is greater. To circumvent this difficulty, i.e., to maximize a

cluster solution, Procedure Relocate of the CLUSTAN 1C program (Wis-

hart, 1975) was applied subsequently to each of the initial cluster

analyses. During each relocation scan, each individual is statisti-

cally removed from its parent cluster and its similarity to all other

clusters is computed. If its similarity to another cluster is greater

the individual is placed in that cluster and the centroids (cluster

centers) are immediately recomputed. The process continues for each

individual for the specified number of scans. An option available

in this procedure allows for the fusion of cluster, thereby reducing

the number of clusters after each relocation scan. The final cluster

array which resulted after Procedure Relocate was felt most likely to

reflect the closest approximation of the actual structure of the data.

The second major problem encountered with the use of clustering

techniques is that of determining when an optimum number of clusters has

been reached. In the heirarchical agglomerative methods each object

initially forms a cluster of one. Objects are joined two at a time

until all objects belong to a single cluster. The decision to halt the

clustering process is largely based on external considerations, there

being no generally accepted test statistic currently available able to

estimate when the number of clusters accurately reflects the underlying

structure of the data. Everitt's (1974) review of current practices

suggests that, while there have been attempts to employ advanced sta-

tistical measures (e.g., minimization of "trace (W)" techniques), most

often the cluster array must be evaluated on the basis of whether the

solution makes heuristic sense, given what is known about the data






73


a priori or upon other external criteria. It is then most prudent to

validate the classification system by examining the relationship of the

clusters to other relevant variables not used in the clustering.















BIBLIOGRAPHY


Applebee, A.N. Research in reading retardation: Two critical problems.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1971, 12, 91-113.

Barr, A.J., Goodnight, J.H., Sall, J.R. and Helwig, J.T. A Users Guide
to SAS 76. Raleigh, North Carolina: SAS Institute, Inc. 1976.

Beery, K.E. Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration Admini-
stration and Scoring Manual. Chicago: Follett Publishing Inc.,
1967.

Benton, A.L. Developmental dyslexia: Neurological aspects. In J.
Friedlander (Ed.) Advances in Neurology, 1975, 7, 1-41.

Blashfield, R.K. Mixture model tests of cluster analysis: Accuracy
of four hierarchical agglomerative methods. Psychological
Bulletin, 1976, 83, 377-388.

Blom, G.E. and Jones, A.W. Bases of classification of reading dis-
orders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 1970, 3, 606-616.

Boder, E. Developmental dyslexia: A diagnostic screening procedure
based on three characteristic patterns of reading and spelling.
Claremont Reading Conference, 1968, 32, 173-187.

Boder, E. Developmental dyslexia: A new diagnostic approach based on
the identification of three subtypes. Journal of School Health,
1970, 40, 289-295.

Boder, E. Developmental dyslexia: Prevailing diagnostic concepts and
a new diagnostic approach. In H.R. Myklebust (Ed.) Progress in
Learning Disability Vol. II. New York: Grune and Stratton, 1971,
293-321.

Camp, B. and Dolcourt, J. Reading and spelling in good and poor
readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 1977, 10(7), 437-443.

Cole, M. and Kraft, M.B. Specific learning disability. Cortex, 1964,
1, 303-313.

DeHirsch, K. and Jansky, J.J. Kindergarten protocols of high achievers,
slow starters and failing readers. In A.H. Keeney and V.T. Keeney
(Eds.) Dyslexia: Diagnosis and Treatment of Reading Disorders.
St. Louis: The C.V. Mosley Company, 1968, 60-67.









Denckla, M.B. Clinical syndromes in learning disabilities: The case
for "splitting" vs. "lumping." Journal of Learning Disabilities,
1972, 5(7), 401-407.

Doehring, D.G. Evaluation of two models of reading disability. In
R. Knights and D. Bakker (Eds.) The Neuropsychology of Learning
Disorders: Theoretical Approaches. Baltimore: University Park
Press, 1977.

Doehring, D.G. and Hoshko, I.M. Classification of reading problems by
the Q-technique of factor analysis, manuscript in preparation, 1977

Dunn, L.M. Expanded Manual for the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test.
Circle Pines, Minnesota: American Guidance Service, Inc., 1965.

Eisenberg, L. Delayed development of speech with special reference to
dyslexia. Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine, 1966,
56, 199-203.

Everitt, B. Cluster Analysis. London: Heinemann Educational Books,
1974.

Gaddes, W.H. Prevalence estimates and the need for definition of
learning disabilities. In R. Knights and D. Bakker (Eds.) The
Neuropsychology of Learning Disorders: Theoretical Approaches.
Baltimore: University Park Press, 1976, 3-24.

Huelsman, C. The WISC subtest syndrome for disabled readers. Per-
ceptual and Motor Skills, 1970, 30, 535-550.

Ingram, T.T.S. Delayed development of speech with special reference
to dyslexia. Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine, 1966,
56, 199-203.

Ingram, T.T.S., Mason, A.W. and Blackburn, I. A retrospective study of
82 children with reading disability. Developmental Medicine and
Neurology, 1970, 12, 271-281.

Jastak, J.F. and Jastak, S.R. The Wide Range Achievement Test Manual
of Instructions, 1976 Revised Edition. Wilmington, Delaware:
Guidance Associates of Delaware, Inc., 1976.

Keeney, A.H. Comprehensive classification of the dyslexias. In A.H.
Keeney and V.T. Keeney (Eds.) Dyslexia: Diagnosis and Treatment
of Reading Disorders. St. Louis: The C.V. Mosley Company, 1968.
174-175.

Kline, C.L. The adolescents with learning problems: How long must
they wait? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 1972, 5, 262-271.

Mattis, S., French, J. and Rapin, I. Dyslexia in children and young
adults, three independent neuropsychological syndromes. Develop-
mental Medicine and Child Neurology, 1975, 17, 150-163.









Monroe, M. Children Who Cannot Read. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press, 1932.

Owen, F.W., Adams, P.A., Forrest, T., Stoltz, S.M. and Fisher, S.
Learning disorders in children: Sibling studies. Monographs of
the Society for Research in Child Development, 1971, 36, 41-61.

Porter, R.D. and Cattell, R.B. Handbook for the Children's Personality
Questionnaire (The CPQ). Champaign, Illinois: Institute for
Personality and Ability Testing, 1972.

Rabinovitch, R.D. Reading problems in children: Definitions and
classifications. In A.H. Keeney and V.T. Keeney (Eds.) Dyslexia:
Diagnosis and Treatment of Reading Disorders. St. Louis: The
C.V. Mosley Company, 1968, 1-10.

Rabinovitch, R.D., Drew, A.L., DeJong, R., Ingram, W. and Whithey, L.A.
A research approach to reading retardation. Association for
Research in Nervous and Mental Disease. 1954, 34, 363-396.

Robins, L.N. Deviant Children Grown Up. Baltimore: Williams and
Wilkins, 1966.

Robinson, H.M. Why Pupils Fail in Reading. Chicago: University of
Chicago Press, 1946.

Rourke, B.P. Brain-behavior relationships in children with learning
disabilities: A research program. American Psychologist, 1975,
30(9), 911-920.

Rourke, B.P. and Finlayson, M.A.J. Neuropsychological significance of
variations in patterns of academic performance: Verbal and visual-
spatial abilities. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 1978,
6(1), 121-133.

Rourke, B.P. and Orr, R.R. Prediction of the reading and spelling
performances of normal and retarded readers: A four-year follow-
up. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 1977, 5, 9-20.

Rourke, B.P., Young, G.C. and Flewelling, R.W. The relationships be-
tween WISC verbal-performance discrepancies and selected verbal,
auditory-perceptual, visual-perceptual and problem-solving
abilities in children with learning disabilities. Journal of
Clinical Psychology, 1971, 27, 475-479.

Rugel, R. and Mitchell, A. Characteristics of familial and non-
familial disabled readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities,
1977, 19(5), 308-313.

Satz, P. Developmental dyslexia: A longitudinal study, unpublished
manuscript, 1977.










Satz, P. and Friel, J. Some predictive antecedents of specific learning
disability: A preliminary one-year follow-up. In P. Satz and
J. Ross (Eds.) The Disabled Learner: Early Detection and Inter-
vention. Rotterdam, The Netherlands: Rotterdam University Press,
1973, 79-98.

Satz, P., Friel, J. and Goebel, R. Some predictive antecedents of
specific reading disability: A three-year follow-up. Bulletin
of the Orton Society, 1975, 25, 91-110.

Satz, P., Friel, J. and Rudegeair, F. Differential changes in the
acquisition of developmental skills in children who later become
dyslexic: A three-year follow-up. In D. Stein and N. Butters
(Eds.) Recovery of Function. New York: Academic Press, 1974.

Satz, P., Taylor, H.G., Friel, J. and Fletcher, J.M. Some develop-
mental and predictive precursors of reading disabilities: A
six-year follow-up. In D. Pearl and A. Benton (Eds.) Dyslexia:
An Appraisal of Current Research. Book in preparation, 1977.

Silver, L.B. Familial patterns in children with neurologically based
learning disabilities. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1971, 4,
349-358.

Silverberg, N. and Silverberg, M. A note on reading tests and their
role in defining reading difficulties. Journal of Learning Dis-
abilities, 1977, 10, 100-103.

Small, N. Levels of perceptual functioning in children: A develop-
mental study. Unpublished Master's Thesis, University of Florida,
1968.

Sokol, R.R. and Sneath, P.H.A. Principles of Numerical Taxonomy.
San Francisco: W.H. Freeman, 1963.

Spreen, 0. and Benton, A. Spreen-Benton Language Examination Profile
(Form A Normal Adults). Iowa City, Iowa: College of Medicine,
University of Iowa, 1965.

Taylor, H.G., Satz, P. and Friel, J. Developmental dyslexia in
relation to other childhood reading disorders: Significance
and clinical utility, manuscript in preparation, 1977.

Van De Riet, V. and Resnick, M.B. An effective model for early child-
hood education. Selected Documents in Psychology. Washington,
D.C.: American Psychological Association, 1973.

Waites, L. World Federation of Neurology: Research group on develop-
mental dyslexia and world illiteracy. Report on Proceedings, 1968,
22, 3-78.

Wechsler, D. Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children.
New York: Psychological Corporation, 1949.









Wechsler, D. Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-
Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation, 1974.

Wiener, M. and Cromer, W. Reading and reading difficulty: A concep-
tual analysis. Harvard Educational Review, 1967, 37, 620-643.

Winer, B.J. Statistical Principles in Experimental Design. New York:
McGraw-Hill, 1971.

Wishart, D. Clustan 1C User Manual. London: Computer Centre,
University of London, 1975.

Wright, P.W. Reading problems and delinquency. Paper presented at
World Congress on Dyslexia, Mayo Clinic, 1974.

Yule, W. and Rutter, M. The epidemiology and social implications of
specific reading retardation. In R. Knights and D. Bakker (Eds.)
The Neuropsychology of Learning Disorders: Theoretical Approaches.
Baltimore: University Park Press, 1976.

Zigler, E. Familial mental retardation: A continuing dilemma. Science,
1967, 155, 292-298. (a)

Zigler, E. Mental retardation. Science, 1967, 157, 577-578. (b)

Zigler, E. Developmental versus difference theories of mental retarda-
tion and the problem of motivation. American Journal of Mental
Deficiency, 1969, 71, 536-556.















BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH

Roy Otto Darby III was born August 22, 1945,in Columbia, South

Carolina. He lived in Savannah, Georgia, Oak Ridge, Tennessee,and

Columbia, South Carolina, where he graduated from A.C. Flora High

School. He was graduated with a B.S. in Psychology from the University

of South Carolina, and was commissioned an Ensign, United States Navy,

in June, 1967. After a tour as Gunnery Officer, Legal Officer, Officer

of the Deck and Command Duty Officer on a destroyer, he was assigned

as Senior Naval Advisor to a Vietnamese Coastal Group (Junk Force).

As a Full Lieutenant he received the Bronze Star, Combat Action Ribbon,

Vietnamese Honor Medal (First Class) and Vietnamese Cross of Gallantry

with Silver Star.

After discharge he entered the University of Florida Graduate

School in Clinical Psychology. He received the Molly Harrower Psycho-

diagnostic Award and the Master of Arts degree in 1974. From 1974 to

1976 he was employed as the psychological evaluator for the Jackson

(Mississippi) Mental Health Center. His residency year was served at

the University of Texas Health Sciences Center at San Antonio. He re-

turned to the University of Florida where he received the Ph.D. degree

in clinical psychology in December, 1978.

He is married to the former Mary McLaurin and has four children,

Christopher Samuel Pace, Michelle Cathryn Darby, Suzanne Michele Pace

and Nan Erin Darby.








I certify that I have read this study and that in my
opinion it conforms to acceptable standards of scholarly
presentation and is fully adequate in scope and quality,
as a dissertation for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy.




Paul Satz, Chafrman
Professor of Clinical Psychology




I certify that I have read this study and that in my
opinion it conforms to acceptable standards of scholarly
presentation and is fully adequate in scope and quality,
as a dissertation for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy.




'acq lin R. Goldman
Professor of Clinical Psychology




I certify that I have read this study and that in my
opinion it conforms to acceptable standards of scholarly
presentation and is fully adequate in scope and quality,
as a dissertation for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy.




Ve.rnon Van De Rfet
Associate Professor of Clinical Psychology




I certify that I have read this study and that in my
opinion it conforms to acceptable standards of scholarly
presentation and is fully adequate in scope and quality,
as a dissertation for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy.




Everette Hall, Jr.
Associate Professor of Psychology









I certify that I have read this study and that in my
opinion it conforms to acceptable standards of scholarly
presentation and is fully adequate in scope and quality,
as a dissertation for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy.




Janet J. Larsen-\^
Associate Professor of Counselor Education




This dissertation was submitted to the Graduate Faculty of the
Department of Psychology in the College of Liberal Arts and
Sciences and to the Graduate Council, and was accepted as partial
fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of
Philosophy.

December 1978




Dean, Graduate School




University of Florida Home Page
© 2004 - 2010 University of Florida George A. Smathers Libraries.
All rights reserved.

Acceptable Use, Copyright, and Disclaimer Statement
Last updated October 10, 2010 - - mvs